ISSN: 2319 – 9695

Ugc-care enlisted.

indian music research papers

Founder's Message

indian music research papers

Dr. Amit Kumar Verma

“Sangeet Galaxy” is a first online Peer-Reviewed Journal dedicated to Indian music. Sangeet Galaxy seeks to publish a balanced mix of high-quality research paper/article from the different domains of Music. ‘Sangeet Galaxy’ journal is eager to develop a strong co-ordination with researchers, academicians and music readers. Our mission is to contribute to the progressof music research by providing free access to research information online without any technical barriers.

It is need of time to create a digital environment to enhance the quality of Music Research. Digital environmentwill be helpful to gey easy access to existing body of knowledge of concerned field. It will also avoid Plagiarism and save time, money and energy of researcher and supervisor both.

Being a high energy, fast moving with strong tendency of action ‘Sangeet Galaxy’ is constantly looking for new ideas and suggestions.

Announcements

Papers invited for january 2025 issue.

Dear Authors,Sangeet Galaxy e-Journal invites […]

Join Telegram Group

Kindly join our Telegram group […]

indian music research papers

Indexing of Journal

We are indexed in.

indian music research papers

Google Scholar

indian music research papers

Library of Congress

Sign up for updates.

Drop you Mail id to get regular updates about Journals

FOCUSED REVIEW article

Music and emotion—a case for north indian classical music.

This article mentions parts of:

Emotional responses to Hindustani raga music: the role of musical structure

  • Read original article

\r\nJeffrey M. Valla

  • Language Literacy and Music Laboratory, National Brain Research Centre, Manesar, India

The ragas of North Indian Classical Music (NICM) have been historically known to elicit emotions. Recently, Mathur et al. (2015) provided empirical support for these historical assumptions, that distinct ragas elicit distinct emotional responses. In this review, we discuss the findings of Mathur et al. (2015) in the context of the structure of NICM. Using, Mathur et al. (2015) as a demonstrative case-in-point, we argue that ragas of NICM can be viewed as uniquely designed stimulus tools for investigating the tonal and rhythmic influences on musical emotion.

Introduction

Music is the art of sound in time, organized to the principles of pitch, rhythm, and harmony ( Limb and Braun, 2008 ). An important function of music is its capacity to communicate emotions ( Tanner and Budd, 1985 ), a view that has been agreed upon by both music performers ( Laukka, 2004 ) and music listeners ( Juslin and Laukka, 2004 ). Indeed, almost all known forms of music have been recognized for their affective emotional qualities ( Goldstein, 1980 ). However, the exact causal mechanisms by which musical sounds generate emotions are still unclear. Current models posit that specific acoustic factors embedded in a music signal exploit the physical environment, the cognitive and perceptual processing systems, and the structure of the auditory system, to generate emotional responses ( Huron, 2006 ; Thompson and Schellenberg, 2006 ).

Though the link between music and emotion has been empirically established ( Juslin and Sloboda, 2011 ), most findings lack generalizability across multi-cultural representations of music. Consequently, while music and emotion studies have standardized the use of Western Classical music as a staple source of stimuli, only a handful have incorporated genres of music native to other cultures. This not only precludes interpretations of universality in musical emotions from their findings, it also overlooks musical stimuli which might have advantages as tools for studying musical emotion ( Thompson and Balkwill, 2010 ). The goal of this review is to make such a case for the unique experimental utility offered by North Indian Classical Music (NICM).

In particular, this review will highlight and expand upon the findings of Mathur et al. (2015) , to demonstrate that NICM comprises of stimuli that not only permit the study of music and emotional response, but are also uniquely designed stimulus tools to investigate how specific psychophysical features like tonality and rhythm modulate musical emotion as separable factors.

North Indian Classical music (NICM), or Hindustani music , is an ancient musical form of India that emerged from a cultural synthesis of the Vedic chant tradition and traditional Persian music ( Kaufmann, 1965 ). The central notion in this system of music are ragas , which are described as musical compositions capable of inducing specific moods or emotions. Past studies have investigated ragas and have shown that distinct ragas elicit distinct emotions ( Balkwill and Thompson, 1999 ; Chordia et al., 2008 ; Wieczorkowska et al., 2010 ). In a study published recently, Mathur et al. (2015) exploited a novel feature of raga stimuli, namely that of different presentation modes, differing in tempo/rhythm but matched in tonal structure, to study music and emotion. They found that when the same raga was presented in distinct presentation modes participants reported elicited emotions with varying levels of arousal. They also found that specific tonal combinations emerged as reliable predictors of emotions that participants reported feeling. These findings indicated that the ragas of NICM not only served as interesting and useful acoustic stimuli that could be exploited to study emotion, but also that the structure of the ragas permitted a systematic, controlled investigation of the role of specific features, namely tonality and rhythm in modulating emotions felt by listeners.

KEY CONCEPT 1. Raga Modal melodies comprising the canon of North Indian Classical Music. Each raga is constructed from five or more musical notes, organized into one ascending sequence, and one descending sequence of notes, which together comprise a single melodic framework. Performance of a raga is restricted within the note sequences of its ascending and descending halves, but is improvised in all other respects (e.g., timing between notes; sustain, attack of each note).

In this review we expand upon these findings, and make the case that NICM is tailor-made for disentangling tonal and temporal influences on musical emotion, and thus an invaluable stimulus tool worth bringing to the attention of researchers in all cultural contexts. Specifically, we will build evidence to support that NICM provides (1) a catalog of systematically varying emotion valence, best reflected in the Circle of Thaats (described below); and (2) a form of musical stimulus which has embedded in its very structure an experimentally controlled manipulation of rhythm and tempo keeping tonality constant, allowing for the disentanglement of tonal from rhythmic influences on emotion.

KEY CONCEPT 2 . Circle of Thaats The Circle of Thaats organizes the ten canonical thaats into a system of incremental variation in tonal ratio (#minor/#Major), with clockwise movement adding Major intervals, and counterclockwise movement subtracting minor intervals. Our lab has previously demonstrated the correlation between emotional valence and tonal ratio in Mathur et al. (2015) . For researchers, the Circle can be used as a “dial” for be systematically and gradually manipulating valence.

The review is organized as follows: We begin with the concepts of consonance and dissonance, one of the primary means by which subjective impressions and emotional responses to music arise predictably from frequency ratios between different notes. We then provide an overview of the NICM system, in which different combinations of consonances and dissonances, in the form of tonal intervals, comprise a canon of melodic themes, as the aforementioned ragas , with prescribed emotional functions. We then segue to an overview of Mathur et al. (2015) , which showed that distinct emotional experiences are reported by listeners for each raga , and expand upon these findings by demonstrating that these inter- raga emotion differences vary systematically and predictably as a function of minor-to-major tonal interval ratios. Finally, we generalize the findings of Mathur et al. to argue that the structure of NICM is well-positioned for empirical studies of the subtleties and universality of emotions communicated through sound.

On a final introductory note, throughout the discussion that follows we refer to musical emotions as being elicited, induced, etc. in listeners, as opposed to using terms like perceived or identified. This choice of terminology is intentional, as the study by Mathur et al. (2015) which motivated this review was explicitly in the latter camp of the debate between cognitivists and emotivists. The cognitivist view is that listeners do not actually feel emotions when they listen to music, they perceive the emotions being expressed ( Kivy, 1989 ). Emotivists, on the other hand, argue that music truly induces emotions, such that a happy tune elicits the same autonomic nervous system responses as any other happy experience ( Scherer and Zentner, 2001 ; Sloboda and Juslin, 2010 ). For a complete overview of how music elicits emotion, see Juslin and Västfjäll (2008) , who provide an extensive review and model for what they argue are the six mechanisms by which music induces emotion: brain stem reflexes, conditioning, visual imagery, contagion, episodic memory, and expectancies fulfilled or denied.

Consonance, Dissonance, and Tonal Intervals—from Quantitative Sound Qualities, to Qualitative Musical Impressions

Once a musical note leaves an instrument or vocal tract, its timbre, and pitch produce minute fluctuations in air pressure around the listener, triggering electrophysiological impulses in the cochlea which then travel through the brain stem and midbrain en route to specialized subregions of the auditory cortex, where they are imbued with emotional interpretation and memory by higher cognitive processes in the orbitofrontal region of the prefrontal cortex ( Zatorre, 2005 ). It is in this way that objective physical changes in an acoustic signal induce psychological effects as subjective and abstract as feelings, turning acoustic features into psychoacoustic phenomena ( Juslin, 1997 ; Laukka et al., 2013 ). Communication of the intended emotion, then, depends upon the musician/composer encoding the emotion in acoustic cues, and the listener successfully decoding these acoustic features from psychophysiological stimulation to emotional meaning. Of the various acoustic cues embedded in music, consonance is the most frequently cited as central to influencing emotion perception. Subjectively speaking, consonance and dissonance describe a level of sweetness/harshness of the sound ( Zentner and Kagan, 1998 ). In terms of the aforementioned encoding/decoding communication between composer and listener, consonance encodes a sense of resolution into a composition, dissonance a sense of unresolved tension ( Limb, 2006 ). Indeed, Kamien (2008 , p. 41) describes consonance and dissonance qualitatively stating that “A stable tone combination is a consonance; consonances are points of arrival, rest, and resolution. An unstable tone combination is a dissonance; its tension demands an onward motion to a stable chord. Thus dissonant chords are “active;” traditionally they have been considered harsh and have expressed pain, grief, and conflict.”

Studies have confirmed the presence of an innate preference for consonance over dissonance, even in infant populations ( Schellenberg and Trehub, 1996 ; Juslin and Zentner, 2002 ). Early investigations of the human auditory system revealed that the human ear can disentangle the harmonic overtones of a series if they are separated by a critical bandwidth ( Plomp and Mimpen, 1968 ). The ability of the auditory nerve fibers to resolve closely spaced frequencies, then, leads to subjective impressions of pleasant sounds or consonance, whereas the inability to clearly resolve closely spaced frequencies resulted in the impression of dissonance or a “harsher” sound ( Von Helmholtz, 1912 ; Plomp and Levelt, 1965 ).

Musically speaking, the range of consonance and dissonance that result from different bandwidths is determined by tonal intervals ( Plomp and Levelt, 1965 ). The tonal interval is determined by two tones, one of which is conventionally the tonic ( Parncutt and Hair, 2011 ). The tonic is the root note around which a musical piece is organized, providing a reference for each tone that is sounded during the performance. Tonal intervals produce impressions of consonance if the frequency differences exceed the critical bandwidth ( Plomp and Levelt, 1965 ).

In Parncutt and Hair's (2011) deconstruction of consonance and dissonance, they argue that the two are not in fact diametrically opposed musical phenomena, as they arise from different relationships between tones in a piece of music, some of which are “vertical,” others “horizontal,” in terms of their placement in staff notation. Simultaneously played tones (e.g., as in a chord) have a vertical relationship, whereas the tonal differences between notes or chords separated temporally (e.g., as in a melody) have a horizontal relationship (p. 139): “In a holistic approach, consonance can be promoted by spectral harmonicity (vertical), harmonic proximity or pitch commonality (horizontal), and familiarity (both vertical and horizontal); dissonance by roughness (vertical) and linear pitch distance (horizontal).” Whilst this less dichotomous definition of consonance and dissonance is somewhat specific to Western music (NICM does not rely on the harmonic progressions of chords, as the tanpura drone serves the purpose of providing the tonic root from which tension/resolution are implied), it does suggest that gauging the consonance/dissonance of NICM ragas is a function of both the relationships between the notes, or swaras , of a raga and the tonic drone, and the relationships between the swaras of a raga in the melodic, horizontal sense.

Tonal intervals form an important organizational principle of musical systems ( Castellano and Krumhansl, 1984 ). When assembled in different combinations, they form diatonic musical modes, the basis of melody construction in any musical system. Due to varying ecological settings, resources, and instrument constructions, some modes used by different musical systems are unique to their native culture ( Perlovsky, 2010 ). Most of our current understanding of the emotions associated with music—emotions presumed to be universal—has come from studies using the modes of Western classical music. Though the literature has consistently shown that listeners associate major and minor modes with positive and plaintive emotions, respectively ( Gagnon and Peretz, 2003 ), increasing work in the field of ethnomusicology suggests that different tonal systems may be able to elicit a subtler gradation of emotions ( Thompson and Balkwill, 2010 ).

North Indian Classical Music

The two dominant genres of Indian music are North Indian Hindustani classical music, and South Indian Carnatic classical music. Whilst the styles of singing, presentation of the notes, emphasis on structure of the musical modes and instruments used in each vary, Hindustani and Carnatic music share many common features, from the raga system, to the use of gamakas (similar to vibrato) and portamento (phrase-leading accents of rapidly increasing pitch; Capwell, 1986 ; Swift, 1990 ). That the arguments made below are made with respect to NICM is due to the fact that the Mathur et al. study from which the data were drawn focused on NICM ragas ; we suspect the same to hold true for Carnatic music as well.

The canon of standard NICM ragas is categorized and organized around a series of heptatonic scales known as thaats (Figure 1 ; Jairazbhoy, 1995 ). In the most widely accepted NICM system, there are 10 thaats consisting of different sequential combinations of 12 notes. Similar to Western Classical Music, the basic set of tones and tonal intervals used in NICM are the 12-tone octave divisions ( Castellano and Krumhansl, 1984 ; Bowling et al., 2012 ). While Western music is based on tones with defined frequencies (e.g., A = 440 Hz) NICM music is constructed from tonal intervals, known as swaras , which are defined in relation to a tonic tone (in practice this tonic takes the form of a drone note, described below). The “major” intervals (i.e., natural notes) are the shuddh swaras while the “minor” intervals are the komal swaras . The tonal intervals are Sa, Re, Ga, Ma, Pa, Dha , and Ni , either in their shush (major) or komal (minor) form, but never both within the same thaat ( Bhatkhande, 1934 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 . Circle of Thaats . The circle of thaats illustrating inter- thaat distance. Nine of the ten thaats on the circle are members of Bhatkhande's classification system ( Bhatkhande, 1934 ). No. 7 thaat Bhairav is not currently used in NICM. Thaat Bhairav with the scale of S r G M P d N is not represented within the circle of thaats (Adapted from Jairazbhoy, 1995 , p. 59).

KEY CONCEPT 3 . Thaat The ten heptatonic scale families which are used to classify the canon of North Indian Classical ragas into tonally similar groups. Not all ragas in a given thaat include every note of that parent thaat , but all ragas in a thaat can be derived from its defining scale.

The fourth natural note, Ma shuddh , has a variant known as the tivr or the augmented fourth (raised by a semitone). Table 1 provides a reference to the shuddh (major) and komal (minor) intervals, and their Western equivalences, for readers versed in Western music theory.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 . Indian/western tonal interval equivalences and frequency ratios.

Across all thaats however, the tonic ( Sa ) and fifth ( Pa ) are considered immutable. Thus theoretically, there can be 32 different thaats , but Bhatkhande's early census and classification of traditional ragas found that the vast majority of ragas can be categorized into 10 families or “ thaats .” The prominent thaats have their names adopted from eminent ragas that are derived of the same mode though the ragas themselves need not necessarily be heptatonic.

It is believed that the most common modes were chosen by Bhatkande such that the structure and practicality were both preserved ( Chordia et al., 2008 ). These 10 canonical thaats , and the relationships between them, are summarized by the Circle of Thaats (Figure 1 , adapted from Jairazbhoy, p. 59). As seen in Figure 1 , since all thaats are heptatonic, as one moves along the circle starting from Bilawal (which has only major notes) in a clockwise manner, the number of minor intervals systematically increases till one reaches a thaat that has no name (but has the highest number of minor intervals). Thus Bilawal and the thaat with no name are diametrically opposite each other. Continuing further, as one move from Todi , the number of minor intervals decreases, and the number of major intervals increases systematically till one reaches Bilawal again.

Thaats provide a useful classification framework, but the core of NICM is the raga ( Bhatkhande, 1934 ; Vatsyayan, 1996 ) The word “ raga ,” which originated in Sanskrit, is defined as “the act of coloring or dyeing;” in this case, the mind and its emotions. The raga was thus conceived as a modal melody capable of eliciting specific emotions, or rasas .

An intrinsic difference between Indian classical and Western classical music is the tonic drone, usually played by a tanpura , provides a reference to the listener (a tonic Sa , often accompanied by a fifth Pa and/or octave Sa ') creating tonal relationships with the “solitary” melody line of the performance. Because the drone is sounded throughout the presentation of the raga , the entire piece can be viewed as a presentation of intervals, not just between notes of the melody line, but between each note and the Sa drone. Table 1 dictates the 12 swaras of Hindustani music displaying their Western classical counterparts, and the frequency ratio of the given note to the tonic.

Tonal Composition

Each raga uses a set of five or more notes from the seven comprising its parent thaat to construct a melody. Multiple ragas are generated from a single thaat , each distinguished by its own signature phrase ( pakar ) and a defined frequency of occurrence of particular notes, vadi being the most prominent note and samvadi being the second most prominent ( Jairazbhoy, 1995 ; Mathur et al., 2015 ). This feature allows two ragas to have the exact same note selection, yet sound different due to varying emphasis on the notes. Bhupali , belonging to thaat Kalyan , and Deskar to thaat Bilawal , are pentatonic ragas and use the notes that are common to both thaats ( Sadhana, 2011 ). Therefore, even while casually interchanged at times, it is important to understand that a raga is not synonymous with a scale; it is a modal melody comprising a defined note selection, differentiated not only on the basis of the notes contained, but also by the frequency of usage of certain notes, the sequencing of ascending ( aarohan ) and descending ( avrohan ) segments, and the pakar ( Kaufmann, 1965 ; Leifer, 1987 ; Jairazbhoy, 1995 ).

The specific combination of tonal intervals in a raga thus create a consonance-dissonance map that then determine which raga will feel pleasant on the ear, and which would fall into areas of dissonance, leading to a harsher sound and the need to be resolved into a consonant interval ( Helmholtz, 1875 ; Zuckerkandl, 1956 ). This subtle combination of tonal intervals permits subtle differences in emotions elicited through music to be investigated using NICM.

Rhythmic Structure

Ragas are usually presented in two consequent sections, the alaap and the gat . The alaap is an elaborate rendition of the various notes of a raga , rendered in free time, introducing and developing the melodic framework, defining characteristics, and mood of a raga . The gat follows the alaap , shifting emphasis to faster sequences of notes, with the accompanying tabla (the main NICM percussion instrument) providing a more explicit rhythmic structure while leaving behind most of the subtleties of pitch articulation. Importantly, the tonal structure of the raga is consistent between alaap and gat , only the tempo is changed. In this way, the raga structure offers an ideal experimental stimulus for disentangling the effects of tempo and tonality: tonality is controlled for between alaap and gat , while rhythm and tempo are manipulated. It is for this reason that NICM was used by Mathur et al. (2015) , enabling the group to isolate the effect of rhythm on emotional elicitationsand, in doing so, demonstrating the unique utility NICM offers as an experimental stimulus.

KEY CONCEPT 4. Alaap and Gat Raga performance has two stages, alaap and gat. The alaap introduces the raga, laying out a tonal framework. The gat introduces the rhythmic accompaniment, increasing in tempo and becoming stricter in rhythmic structure until there is very little room left for improvisation. As both stages use the same scale, changing tempo, ragas are experimental stimuli by nature, with which melodic and temporal effects on emotion can be distinguished.

Cultural Relationship between Raga, Rasa , and Bhava (Modal Melody, Mood, and Emotion Label)

On a more subjective level, emotional intent is a distinguishing feature of the NICM raga system. Whereas emotions and moods are implied characteristics of Western Classical music, Indian ragas have prescribed emotional effects, or rasa s ( Vatsyayan, 1996 ), each rasa intended to alter the mood ( bhava ) of the listener in a particular manner. Erotic love ( sringara ), patheticness ( karuna ), devotion ( bhakti ), comedy ( hasya ), horror ( bhayanaka ), repugnancy ( bibhatsa ), heroism ( vira ), fantastical, furious ( roudra ), and peaceful ( shanta ) were named in Bhatkhande's description of rasa and bhava ( Bhatkhande, 1934 ; Bowling et al., 2012 ).

Knowing the bhava that the rasa of a particular raga is meant to induce ( Mathur et al., 2015 ), such stimuli are invaluable to musical emotion studies, cross-cultural or otherwise, as the Circle of Thaats , and the canon of standard ragas it encompasses, can be utilized as a catalog for eliciting subtle gradations in emotional effect, some of which are culturally universal, others less so. Whilst Western listeners perceive the same basic emotions—happy, sad, angry, disgusted, surprised, fearful—as native listeners in Hindustani music, more subtle emotional gradations of basic emotions (e.g., “peacefulness” rather than happiness) are more easily identified by native listeners ( Balkwill and Thompson, 1999 ; William Forde Balkwill et al., 2004 ; Fritz et al., 2009 ; Laukka et al., 2013 ).

Mathur et al. (2015)

Recently, Mathur et al. (2015) tested the hypothesis that ragas elicit distinct emotional feelings. Using 3-min compositions of 12 ragas , presented in the form of an online survey, participants rated these ragas on the degree to which they elicited different emotions. All ragas were composed by a professional musician and rendered on sarod , an Indian stringed instrument.

As indicated earlier, Mathur et al. exploited the structure of a raga composition and presented each of the 12 ragas in both alaap and gat . Participants were instructed to rate each excerpt on eight distinct emotions on a 0–4 Likert scale (with 0 being “not at all felt” to 4 being “felt the most”) for each of the following emotion labels: happy, romantic, devotional, calm/soothed, angry, longing/yearning, tensed/restless, and sad. The study did not use a forced choice task but instead sought each raga to be rated for each of the eight emotions, sensitive to the fact that a single musical composition can elicit multiple moods. Specifically the study sought to determine if participant responses (1) differed between the emotions experienced by alaap and gat for various ragas (2) whether the psychophysical variables of rhythm and tonality influenced the emotions experienced.

The first finding of Mathur et al.'s study was that distinct ragas are associated with distinct emotional elicitations. This was similar to the findings reported by Balkwill and Thompson (1999) which showed that even western listeners who were unfamiliar with the tonal system of NICM perceived the intended emotion in ragas . However Balkwill and Thompson used ragas only in the alaap mode and implemented forced choice task. Participants in that study were required to indicate which of the four target emotions was dominant for the raga. Mathur et al. on the other hand asked each participant to rate the extent to which each of the emotions were experienced during the listening of the raga . Mathur et al. also found that that when the raga was presented in “ alaap ,” participants ratings were either calm (positive) or sad (negative) emotion. However, when presented in the gat condition, a finer discrimination of emotions were elicited (happy, romantic, calm) and (sad, longing, tension). This was the first experimental verification of the hypothesis that distinct emotions are associated with alaap and gat of a raga . Further, since Mathur et al. also used acoustic analysis to extract estimates of rhythmic regularity and tempo, they correlated acoustic features with behavioral ratings of emotional elicitation and were able to demonstrate that high arousal emotions like happy/tensed were associated with gat . As elaborated earlier the gat follows faster sequences of notes and provides an explicit rhythmic structure. A comparison of these results with those from the study conducted by Balkwill and Thompson showed that tempo and melodic complexity had some predictive power. However this was found only for some differences. Balkwill and Thompson (1999) used psychophysical ratings of tempo and melodic complexity and found that a combination of the two, predicted emotions primarily joy and sadness. Similar results were also reported by Gabrielsson and Juslin (1996) who showed that faster tempo were associated with positive emotions while slower tempo with negative emotions.

What was novel in Mathur et al.'s study was the finding that there is a change in the level of arousal between alaap and gat for the same raga. Since the tonal structure of the raga is preserved between alaap and gat , only the tempo is changed and the finding that high arousal emotions are associated with gat points to the fact that the raga structure is an optimal stimulus to dissociate the effects of tempo and tonality: tonality is controlled for between alaap and gat , while rhythm and tempo are manipulated. This result from Mathur et al. (2015) , illustrated that this unique structure of NICM that enables the isolation of the effect of rhythm on emotion elicitationrenders it as a useful experimental stimulus.

Of greater interest was the second primary finding of Mathur et al. (2015) study which showed that specific tonic intervals were robust predictors of elicited emotions. Major intervals were found to be associated with positive emotions and minor intervals to be associated with negative emotions. An analysis of tonal intervals of ragas , revealed that ragas rated as positive (such as “calm” and “happy”) had a greater mean frequency of occurrence of major intervals ( shuddh swaras ) whereas ragas with negative emotion (e.g., sad or tensed) were characterized by an increased frequency of minor intervals ( Komal swaras ). Figure 2 shows a distribution of mean frequency of occurrence of tonic intervals for the 12 ragas used in the study. Red bars represent the mean frequency of occurrence of shuddh swaras whereas that of komal swaras is represented with blue bars for each raga . To the best of our knowledge this finding for ragas is novel and has not been reported earlier.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 . Tonality for ragas . The above figure represents the tonic interval distribution for the 12 ragas used in the study ( Mathur et al., 2015 ). The tonal distribution of ragas rated as “calm” is represented with red background color panel whereas the tonal distribution of ragas rated as “sad” is represented with blue background color panel. Within each panel the mean frequency of occurrence of shuddh swaras [S ( Sa ), R ( Re ), G ( Ga ), M ( Ma ), P ( Pa ), D ( Dha ), N ( Ni )] is depicted with red bars whereas the mean frequency of occurrence of komal swaras [r( re ), g( ga ), m( ma ), d( dha ), n( ni )] is depicted with blue bars.

To further explore the findings from Mathur et al. and assess the degree to which emotion ratings agree with rasa variation around the Circle of Thaats , we first associated a valence score with each raga , which is a difference in the ratings of the two highest experienced emotions, calm and sad. As a consequence, a value >0 is associated with positive valence whereas, a difference < 0 is associated with negative valence. Next, we define a tonal ratio, which is the ratio of the number of minor intervals (m) to major intervals (M) for each raga . The valence score and tonal ratios estimated are listed in Table 2 .

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 . Ratios of minor (m) to major (M) intervals along with mean ratings for ragas belonging to each thaat as estimated in Mathur et al. (2015) .

In Figure 3 , we compare the tonal ratios with the valence score of the ragas used by Mathur et al. The tonal ratios and valence score as estimated for various ragas are represented along the Circle of Thaats (refer to Table 2 and Figure 3 ). The thaats for which emotional elicitations for more than one raga were available, an average valence score has been estimated (Table 2 ). The tonal ratios are expressed on a color scale (red to blue) while the average valence score associated with each thaat is indicated along with the name of the respective thaat . Figure 3 reveals that ragas belonging to thaat Bilawal (tonal ratio 0.00), elicits emotions with positive valence (e.g., valence score of raga Hansadhwani and Tilak Kamod are 1.22 and 1.28, respectively) where ragas belonging to thaat Todi (tonal ratio 0.57) and thaat Marwa (tonal ratio 0.29) evokes emotions of negative valence (e.g., valence score of Basant Mukhari or Mivan ki Todi is −0.35 and −0.33, respectively). Thus as the tonal ratio systematically increases moving clockwise from 0 and subsequently decreases, valence follows suit. In effect, the qualitative rasa variation adumbrated by the Circle of Thaats aligns with quantitative variations in both emotion rating valence and tonal ratio for the ragas tested.

KEY CONCEPT 5. TONAL RATIO The ratio of #minor/#Major intervals in a raga. As minor intervals are dissonant and Major intervals are consonant, this ratio gauges consonance/dissonance across an entire raga scale. Raga tonal ratios align with valences of participants' subjective ratings, suggesting the canon of North Indian ragas is a source for experimental stimuli with for those seeking a range and gradation of valences.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 . Similarities between the tonal ratio (m/M) and valence of ragas when arranged along Circle of Thaats . The tonal ratio is color coded in increasing order on a scale of red to blue. Valence values associated with each thaat are indicated next to the names of the thaat , with negative valence preceded by the minus sign. For thaats where multiple ragas were included in Mathur et al. a mean valence was estimated based on the ratings in Table 2 .

Finally, a third finding of Mathur et al. (2015) was that out of the 12 tonic intervals, the minor second interval ( komal re ) was the best predictor of negative valence. As seen in Figure 3 , there are two locations along the circle of thaats , which we refer to as transition thaats where a change in tonal ratio is accompanied by a change in valence category (refer to Figure 3 and Table 2 ). These are Kafi to Asavari (positive to negative valence) and Purvi to Khamaaj (negative to positive). While symmetrically located on the circle of thaats , and similar tonal ratios the subsequent valence associated with the transition thaats is quite different. We attribute this to the specific minor intervals involved.

While the minor intervals present in transition thaats Kafi to Asavari are minor third ( komal ga ) and minor seventh ( komal ni ) those present in transition thaats Purvi to Marwa are minor second ( komal re ) and tritone ( tivra ma ) respectively. We suggest that the presence of the minor second in the transition thaats Purvi and Marwa leads to their association with higher negative valence score (−0.26 and −0.42, respectively) as compared to thaat Kafi (0.50). While the study by Mathur et al. did not include a raga from thaat Asvari the results encourage us to hypothesize that the minor second serves a crucial role in conveying negative valence. Further studies should attempt to investigate its role in detail by sampling a larger representation of ragas from each thaat .

The purpose of this review was to demonstrate NICM ragas as robust stimuli capable of eliciting distinct, predictable emotions, with tonal relationships and rhythmic tempo influencing the valence and strength of emotional effects in the listener. The ragas used in Mathur et al. (2015) were only 12 in number, but since they had been sampled across almost all thaats we attempted to speculate how the structure of the tonic intervals might predict the emotional valence associated with a raga .

Moving around the Circle of Thaats , emotional valence systematically varied along with the tonal ratios of each thaat . In this way, music emotion researchers may find experimental utility in the Circle of Thaats, as a catalog of stimuli varying in degrees of valence not only systematically, but incrementally , in the sense of finer gradations of valence than the more binary notions of “positive”/”negative” “happy”/”sad” typically ascribed to consonance and dissonance effects on emotion. In addition, built into the very structure of Indian compositions is an experimental manipulation of rhythmic tempo between alaap and gat , keeping tonal intervals constant, which in Mathur et al. (2015) revealed that the musical differences between sadness and tension, calmness and happiness may be more a function of rhythm than melody.

In sum, the catalog of systematically and incrementally varying emotional valence comprising the Circle of Thaats ; and a varying rhythmic structure which controls for tonality across a single raga , together make NICM music an invaluable auditory stimulus, tailor made and uniquely useful for experimentally controlled studies of musical emotion. We acknowledge that at present the preliminary results discussed here are speculative and require more detailed investigation. Since the tonic ratio is directly related to emotional response, further studies should also probe the nature of this relationship in influencing the strength of arousal of positive or negative valence of a raga , a feature that is often adopted by various performing artistes that has not been experimentally investigated. Future research would also do well to test the degree to which the constant tonic drone amplifies the strength of the emotional valences induced via the consonances and dissonances of these tonic ratios.

Finally, whilst a main aim of this review was to describe why ragas are a uniquely useful experimental stimuli for studies of music and emotion, this methodological prescription comes with an important caveat. Ragas are musical stimuli with deep, specific cultural origins and associations ( Wieczorkowska et al., 2010 ). But although they have been shown to elicit culturally-specific emotions which appear to be lost on non-native listeners, they also convey emotions that are shared between native and non-native listeners ( Laukka et al., 2013 ). Consequently, for experiments using raga stimuli for cross-cultural research this is crucial to note, as beyond universal emotions there are enculturated emotions elicited by culture-specific cues in music. For studies using only Western or only Indian samples, however, such cultural effects should not be a concern, as all participants would be equally advantaged or disadvantaged in identifying culturally dependent musical cues.

On a final, related point, it is important to note that the usage of ragas in the Western music cognition literature is nearly always in the context of cross-cultural differences. We hope Western readers come away from this review with an understanding that ragas can be thought of as more than “world music,” and useful for more than only cross-cultural studies of music cognition and emotion.

Ultimately, we hope this review brings the unique experimental value of NICM to the attention of music emotion researchers, useful for investigating emotional elicitations to music, within, between, and across different cultures.

Author Contributions

NS: designed the study and wrote the paper; JV and JA: contributed to paper writing; AM: collected data, conducted analysis, and contributed in writing the paper.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Funding for this work was provided by National Brain Research Centre, India. We thank Pt. Mukesh Sharma for composing the stimuli for the study. We thank our volunteers for their participation in the survey. We thank Hymavathy Balasubramanian for feedback during manuscript editing. We thank Hymavathy Balasubramanian for feedback during manuscript editing.

Author Biography

Balkwill, L. L., and Thompson, W. (1999). A cross-cultural investigation of emotion in music: psychophysical and cultural cues. Music Percept. Interdisciplinary J. 17, 43–64. doi: 10.2307/40285811

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balkwill, L. L., Thompson, W. F., and Matsunaga, R. I. E. (2004). Recognition of emotion in Japanese, western, and Hindustani music by Japanese listeners. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 46, 337–349. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5584.2004.00265.x

Bhatkhande, V. N. (1934). A Short Historical Survey of the Music of Upper India . Chicago, IL: BSSukthankar.

Bowling, D. L., Sundararajan, J., Han, S., and Purves, D. (2012). Expression of emotion ineastern and western music mirrors vocalization. PLoS ONE 7:e31942. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031942

Capwell, C. (1986). Musical life in nineteenth-century Calcutta as a component in the history of a secondary urban center. Asian Music 18, 139–163. doi: 10.2307/834162

Castellano, M. A., Bharucha, J. J, and Krumhansl, C. L. (1984). Tonal hierarchies in the music of North India. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 113, 394–412. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.113.3.394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chordia, P., Godfrey, M., and Rae, A. (2008). “Extending content-based recommendation: the case of Indian classical music,” in Proceeding of International Conference on Music Information Retrieval (Atlanta, GA), 571–576.

Google Scholar

Gabrielsson, A., and Juslin, P. N. (1996). Emotional expression in music performance: between the performer's intention and the listener's experience. Psychol. Music 24, 68–91. doi: 10.1177/0305735696241007

Fritz, T., Jentschke, S., Gosselin, N., Sammler, D., Peretz, I., Turner, R., et al. (2009). Universal recognition of three basic emotions in music. Curr. Biol. 19, 573–576. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.058

Gagnon, L., and Peretz, I. (2003). Mode and tempo relative contributions to “happy-sad” judgments in equitone melodies. Cogn. Emot. 17, 25–40. doi: 10.1080/02699930302279

Goldstein, A. (1980). Thrills in response to music and other stimuli. Physiol. Psychol . 8, 126–129. doi: 10.3758/BF03326460

Helmholtz, H. V. (1875). VON. 1954. On the Sensations of Tone . Transl. by Alexander Ellis in Chicago. New York: NY: Dover Publications.

Huron, D. (2006). Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Jairazbhoy, N. A. (1995). The Raags of North Indian Music: Their Structure and Evolution, 2nd Edn . Mumbai: Popular Press Pvt. Ltd.

Juslin, P. N. (1997). Emotional communication in music performance: a functionalist perspective and some data. Music Percept. 14, 383–418. doi: 10.2307/40285731

Juslin, P. N., and Laukka, P. (2004). Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: a review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening. J. New Music Res. 33, 217–238. doi: 10.1080/0929821042000317813

Juslin, P. N., and Sloboda, J. A. (2011). Handbook of Music and Emotions: Theory, Research, Applications . Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Juslin, P. N., and Västfjäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: the need to consider underlying mechanisms. 31, 559–575; discussion: 575–621. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08005293

Juslin, P. N., and Zentner, M. R. (2002). Current trends in the study of music and emotion: overture. Music. Sci. 5, 3–21. doi: 10.1177/10298649020050S101

Kamien, R. (2008). Music: An Appreciation, 6th Edn . New York, NY: McGraw Hill.

Kaufmann, W. (1965). Rasa, raaga-maalaa and performance times in North Indian Raagas. Ethnomusicology 9, 272–291. doi: 10.2307/850238

Kivy, P. (1989). Sound Sentiment: An Essay On the Musical Emotions, Including the Complete Text of the Corded Shell . Chicago, IL: Temple University Press.

Laukka, P. (2004). Instrumental music teachers' views on expressivity: a report from music conservatories. Music Educ. Res. 6, 45–56. doi: 10.1080/1461380032000182821

Laukka, P., Eerola, T., Thingujam, N. S., Yamasaki, T., and Beller, G. (2013). Emotion universal and culture-specific factors in the recognition and performance of musical affect expressions. Emotion 13:234. doi: 10.1037/a0031388

Leifer, L. (1987). “Raga and tala: a western view,” in Aspects of Indian Music, A Collection of Essays , ed S. Mutatkar (New Delhi: Sangeet Natak Akademi), 18–28.

Limb, C. J. (2006). Structural and functional neural correlates of music perception. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell. Evol. Biol. 288, 435–446. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20316

Limb, C. J., and Braun, A. R. (2008). Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: an fMRI study of jazz improvisation. PLoS ONE 3:e1679. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001679

Mathur, A., Vijayakumar, S. H., Chakrabarti, B., and Singh, N. C. (2015). Emotional responses to Hindustani raga music: the role of structure. Front. Psychol. 6:513. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00513

Parncutt, R., and Hair, G. (2011). Consonance and dissonance in music theory and psychology: disentangling dissonant dichotomies. J. Interdisciplinary Music Stud . 5, 119–166. doi: 10.1177/2041669516655812

Perlovsky, L. (2010). Musical emotions: functions, origins, evolution. Phys. Life Rev. 7, 2–27. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2009.11.001

Plomp, R., and Levelt, W. J. (1965). Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 38, 548–560. doi: 10.1121/1.1909741

Plomp, R., and Mimpen, A. M. (1968). The ear as a frequency analyzer. II. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 43, 764–767. doi: 10.1121/1.1910894

Sadhana, A. (2011). Sadhana's Raag Hindustani . Available online at: http://raag-hindustani.com (Accessed on March 1, 2016).

Schellenberg, E. G., and Trehub, S. E. (1996). Natural musical listeners: evidence from infant listeners. Psychol. Sci. 7, 5–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00373.x

Scherer, K. R., and Zentner, M. R. (2001). “Emotional effects of music: production rules,” in Music and Emotion: Theory and Research , eds P. Juslin and J. Sloboda (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 361–392.

Sloboda, J. A., and Juslin, P. N. (2010). “At the interface between the inner and outer world,” in Handbook of Music and E: Theory, Research, and Applications , eds P. N. Juslin J. A. Sloboda (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 73–98.

Swift, G. (1990). Ornamentation in south Indian music and the violin. J. Soc. Asian Music 21, 17–18. doi: 10.2307/834112

Tanner, M., and Budd, M. (1985). Understanding music. Proc. Aristotelian Soc. Suppl. 59, 215–248. doi: 10.1093/aristoteliansupp/59.1.215

Thompson, W. F., and Balkwill, L. L. (2010). “Cross-cultural similarities and differences,” in Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, and Applications , eds P. N. Juslin and J. A. Sloboda (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Thompson, W. F., and Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). “Listening to music,” in MENC Handbook of Musical Cognition and Development , ed R. Colwell (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 72–123.

Vatsyayan, K. (1996). Bharata The Natyasastra. Sahitya Akademi . Available online at: http://books.google.com/books?id=_fdsox_H1bQC&pgis=1

Von Helmholtz, H. (1912). On the Sensations of Tone as a Physiological Basis for the Theory of Music . London: Longmans; Green.

Wieczorkowska, A. A., Datta, A. K., Sengupta, R., Dey, N., and Mukherjee, B. (2010). “On search for emotion in Hindusthani vocal music,” in Advances in Music Information Retrieval , eds Z. W. Ras and A. Wieczorkowska (Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer), 285–304.

Zatorre, R. (2005). Music, the food of neuroscience? Nature 434, 312–315. doi: 10.1038/434312a

Zentner, M. R., and Kagan, J. (1998). Infants' perception of consonance and dissonance in music. Infant Behav. Dev. 21, 483–492. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90021-2

Zuckerkandl, V. (1956). Sound and Symbol . Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Keywords: raga , emotion, tonic ratio, thaat , music

Citation: Valla JM, Alappatt JA, Mathur A and Singh NC (2017) Music and Emotion—A Case for North Indian Classical Music. Front. Psychol . 8:2115. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02115

Received: 07 January 2017; Accepted: 20 November 2017; Published: 19 December 2017.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2017 Valla, Alappatt, Mathur and Singh. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Relevant areas of research in Indian Musicology today

Profile image of Keshavchaitanya Kunte

2018, International Inventive Multidisciplinary Journal

This paper takes a review of traditional avenues in the Indian Music research and further, enlists the possible avenues open for Music research in Indian perspective in the contemporary scenario.

Related Papers

Bisakha Goswami

indian music research papers

Asian Quarterly

Keshavchaitanya Kunte

From ancient times till date, the language of the song in India has undergone many changes. The song texts in the ancient period were primarily Sanskrit and Prakrit. In the medieval period, due to the development of regional languages, the importance of Sanskrit as a language of the song started diminishing. Sanskrit is still in vogue as song language in Carnatic music. But it is not the case in Hindustani Art music. Except for Ashtapadi, the song-texts of major song-forms such as Dhrupad, Khayal, Tappa, Thumri are chiefly in regional dialects such as Brij, Awadhi, Mewadi, Saraiki-Panjabi, etc. In the modern period, few composers have composed Dhrupad, Khayal, and also line-components for Tarana or Chaturang in Sanskrit. One also finds the influence of Sanskrit in some Dhrupad and Khayal compositions. This paper firstly takes a brief review of Sanskrit compositions of Hindustani Art music through the ages, and secondly discusses some questions such as why Sanskrit lost the pivotal place in song-texts in Hindustani Art music and still why modern composers, though few, have tried composing Sanskrit Bandish-s. The paper also ponders upon some issues such as potentialities and limitations of any language for being a song-language, and how vocalists handle the song-texts in actual rendition.

A Metaphorical Study on Teaching Individual Creativity in Asia: Indian Ālāp and Vietnamese Dạo

Chinthaka P Meddegoda

International Research Journal of MMC

Parsuram P Poudel

Varieties of music instruments are available today to produce the audible sounds which are used to make a piece of music. The music instruments have been classified into various groups depending upon the size, materials, usages, structure, and others. But, today many new practices, experiments, devices, and new instruments have been emerged with the flow of time in musical field. For all the concerned with music, a neo-classification of instruments is ever raised a question. So, from the educational point of view, some of the new music instruments, which are played today, further should be re-classified for convenience. The present article is based on an overview upon the study about the music instruments by following the secondary data and analytical method. After interpretation and analysis, some music instruments, which are still unclassified are categorized under neo-classification.

Christine Guillebaud

En 1964, Alan P. Merriam définissait l’ethnomusicologie comme l’étude de la musique dans la culture et en tant que culture («music in and as culture»)1. Or, en Asie du Sud, rares sont les auteurs qui ont pensé l’objet musical en ces termes. Les pages qui suivent font l’état des lieux des travaux les plus importants des quinze dernières années. J’aborderai successivement la musicologie historique, les études portant sur les traditions classiques contemporaines, l’ethnomusicologie naissante des objets «non classiques», les modèles performatifs et enfin les enjeux soulevés par l’étude des musiques dites «populaires» (popular music).

Birgit Abels

Asian Music 52(1), 30-56

Hindustani khayal music is a prominent vocal classical genre in northern India. Scholarship on khayal, elaborating on its construction as national and classical, has often rendered its social experiences in manners that suggest homogeneity across regions and quasi-autonomy from popular music. Focusing on a contemporary instance—a television show that features Marathi popular songs—this article unpacks the relationship of khayal with popular music in the state of Maharashtra. In the process, the article reflects on the meanings that khayal has accrued in a regional context as well as on the nature of music categories, genres, and their calibration.

Dr. Subroto Mihir alias Suvratadev Sharmana Vandyopadhyay Roy

As opposed to the reductionist views of time which include a linear approach, the Indian is a cyclic conception of time which events follow. According to this view the super-structure of time is cyclic; tangible and intangible events keep repeating in smaller cycles. The essence of Creation at a macro level and creativity at the micro or individual level is the fact that they appear differently to us. In north Indian art music (NIAM), I propose that Avartan is its “cyclicity” which is responsible for creativity. The heavy demand for extempore improvisation in NIAM, forces a performer to continually view immediate past musical act and improvise based on this. In this manner, the spiral of creativity is generated. Then, at a point of time one wants to break away from this cycle just as an aeroplane leaves its runway after gaining momentum. At a very secular level, this process replicates an urge for freedom from routines in life, but one cannot value freedom without being shackled and finding a tool to gain a threshold frequency for final release. Avartan as a tool provides a process of observation, retention, and recall; itself is a cyclical process. At an esoteric level, in India, the timeless Siva is the chief deity of time and hence He devours it. He is the Yogi (without categories/unity itself) blissful between two moments and full of potential creative energy. This situation is replicated in the sam of Avartan or the first beat (unity) in the cyclic meter. Sam is the pivotal aspect which is continually sought after by the artiste in an attempt to meet the Yogi sitting between moments. From this unity multiplicity is born giving birth to cyclic meter representing the infinitely diverse phenomenal world.

Gisa Jähnichen

Taking organological research and display of outcomes in China, Vietnam, and Malaysia as example, this paper is to highlight burning issues regarding the purpose and meaning of the discipline in the context of research communities experiencing urbanity. Methodologically, this paper argues mainly in dialogue with the discussion on transcultural musicology. In the mentioned region, urban museums, archives, and universities are widely modeled according to successful academic institutions of supposedly global importance. The perspective of expected success needs a radical turn in order to serve social sustainability and a growing knowledge base that is inclusive regarding subjects and objects under research. The radical turn in de-academizing derives from the questioning of basic assumptions that once started with an uncritical praise of Herder's groundwork in defining 'people' and finds expression in the social engagement with ethnic minorities, migrants , diaspora, and other categorically constructed groups of people in specific nation states and their urban centers.

Towards the History of Ideas in Ethnomusicology: Theory and Methods between the Late 18th and the Early 20th Century

Ulrich Morgenstern

The history of ethnomusicology is the history of ideas and concepts of why and how to deal with expressive practices in social formations which are usually located outside the researcher's primary cultural experience. Ideas in ethnomusicology (comparative musicology, anthropology of music, folk music research, folkloristics) are interlinked with other scholarly disciplines and academic fields. The history of the field is sometimes described as a shift from "[…] the history of the field is far more extensive than many had assumed or admitted".

Loading Preview

Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.

RELATED PAPERS

Matt Rahaim

Anthony Seeger

Luboš Chroustovský

Zdravko Blazekovic

Luzia A U R O R A Rocha

Pedro Bento

Música em Contexto

Christina Homer

Sarah Deters

Society for Ethnomusicology Newsletter 43(4)

Peter J Hoesing

Antonio Baldassarre

Katherine Butler Schofield

Standing Committee for the Humanities (SCH) Discussion Paper

Julia Craig-McFeely

Amparo Fontaine

Bernd Brabec

Ruy Câmara Câmara

SibE-Sociedad de Etnomusicología

Enrique Cámara de Landa , Leonardo Díaz Collao

Thor Magnusson

Simone Krüger Bridge

Sidney Monteiro

Jeremy Montagu

Interdisciplinary Science Reviews

Patrick M Egan

Luis Felipe Oliveira

Adrian McNeil

Journal of New Music Research

National University of Singapore, Asia Research Institute, Feb 27-28, 2020

Guy L Beck , CAROLA ERIKA LOREA , Tay Minghua

LAURA ZATTRA

Journal of the American Musical Instrument Society

Gabriele Rossi Rognoni

Ecomusicology Newsletter

Andrew Mark

Live-Electronic Music. Composition, Performance and Study

Emmanuel Obed Acquah

Ortensia Giovannini

The Record News

Suresh Chandvankar

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

A Comparative Study of Audio Source Separation Techniques on Indian Classical Music: Performance Evaluation and Analysis

  • Conference paper
  • First Online: 26 May 2024
  • Cite this conference paper

indian music research papers

  • Yajna Pandith 12 ,
  • H. Pavan 12 ,
  • T. H. Abhishek 12 &
  • B. Ashwini 12  

Part of the book series: Lecture Notes in Networks and Systems ((LNNS,volume 946))

Included in the following conference series:

  • International Conference on Smart Computing and Communication

108 Accesses

Music, a rich form of auditory expression, encompasses a diverse array of sounds, broadly classified into two main categories: Harmonics and Percussions. Harmonic sounds, distinguished by their distinct pitches, give rise to melodies and chords, while Percussive sounds, produced by instruments like drums and snares, introduce sudden pitch changes. Real-world music compositions often combine these two classes, creating challenges in processing and analysis. This research’s primary goal is to develop and compare effective algorithms for separating audio, into two distinct components, the isolating harmonic component and the percussive component. Exploiting the localization properties of these sound classes, with percussive sounds in time and harmonic sounds in frequency, it aims to achieve precise separation. This separation finds practical use in music remixing, key signature detection, instrument transcription, chord identification, and rhythmic analysis. The paper delves into theoretical foundations, implementation details, and comprehensive spectrogram analyses to demonstrate the approach’s effectiveness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Müller M (2015) Fundamentals of music processing. Springer, Heidelberg. ISBN: 978-3-319-21944-8

Google Scholar  

Fitzgerald D (2010) Harmonic/percussive separation using median filtering. In: 13th international conference on digital audio effects (DAFx-10)

Quesada Canadas F, Vera-Candeas P, Ruiz Reyes N, Carabias-Orti J, Cabañas P (2014) Percussive/harmonic sound separation by non-negative matrix factorization with smoothness/sparseness constraints. EURASIP J Audio Speech Music Process https://doi.org/10.1186/s13636-014-0026-5

López-Serrano P, Dittmar C, Özer Y, Müller M (2019) Music processing applications of nonnegative matrix factorization. In: Proceedings of the 22nd international conference on digital audio effects (DAFx-19), Birmingham, UK, 2–6 Sept 2019

Ono N, Miyamoto K, Le Roux J, Kameoka H, Sagayama S (2008) Separation of a monaural audio signal into harmonic/percussive components by complementary diffusion on spectrogram. In: Proceedings of the European Signal Processing Conference (EUSIPCO)

Toshkana B, Ramakrishnan KR (2012) A modified spectrogram diffusion algorithm for improved harmonic/percussive separation in music signals. In: International conference on Signal Processing and Communications (SPCOM)

FitzGerald D, Liukus A, Rafii Z, Pardo B, Daudet L (2014) Harmonic, percussive separation using Kernel additive modelling. In: 25th IET Irish Signals & Systems Conference, (2014) ISSC 2014/CIICT 2014. Limerick 2014, pp 35–40. https://doi.org/10.1049/cp.2014.0655

Dittmar C, López-Serrano, Müller M (2018) Unifying local and global methods for harmonic-percussive source separation. In: 2018 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP), Calgary, AB, pp 176–180. https://doi.org/10.1109/ICASSP.2018.8462119

Kim H-G, Moreau N, Sikora T (2005) MPEG-7 audio and beyond: audio content indexing and retrieval. Wiley

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

PES University, Bangalore, India

Yajna Pandith, H. Pavan, T. H. Abhishek & B. Ashwini

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yajna Pandith .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Faculty of Engineering, University of the Ryukyus, Nishihara, Okinawa, Japan

Tomonobu Senjyu

College of Computing, Khon Kaen University, Khon Kaen, Thailand

Chakchai So–In

Global Knowledge Research Foundation, Ahmedabad, Gujarat, India

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2024 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this paper

Cite this paper.

Pandith, Y., Pavan, H., Abhishek, T.H., Ashwini, B. (2024). A Comparative Study of Audio Source Separation Techniques on Indian Classical Music: Performance Evaluation and Analysis. In: Senjyu, T., So–In, C., Joshi, A. (eds) Smart Trends in Computing and Communications. SmartCom 2024 2024. Lecture Notes in Networks and Systems, vol 946. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-97-1323-3_10

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-97-1323-3_10

Published : 26 May 2024

Publisher Name : Springer, Singapore

Print ISBN : 978-981-97-1322-6

Online ISBN : 978-981-97-1323-3

eBook Packages : Intelligent Technologies and Robotics Intelligent Technologies and Robotics (R0)

Share this paper

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

The PMC website is updating on October 15, 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • v.15(11); 2023 Nov
  • PMC10754644

Logo of cureus

Impact of Listening to Indian Classical Music, or Rāgas, on the Electroencephalogram: A Meta-Analysis

Shrinit babel.

1 Morsani College of Medicine, University of South Florida, Florida, USA

Suman Baral

2 Surgery, Lumbini Medical College and Teaching Hospital, Tansen, NPL

Abhishek Srivastava

3 Center for Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Kokilaben Dhirubhai Ambani Hospital and Medical Research Institute, Mumbai, IND

Ancient Indian classical music (ICM) has long been lauded and recognized for influencing emotional responses by influencing the human body's resonance. A meta-analysis of prospective case studies published in the last ten years on the effect of ancient Indian music rāgas on brain waves is investigated. This meta-analysis aimed to analyze published prospective studies investigating the effect of ancient Indian rāgas on EEG in healthy subjects.

The present study included prospective studies published since 2012. Studies were obtained by searching four databases, such as PsychINFO, PubMed, Google Scholar, and JSTOR, and searching related journals. Eligibility criteria included studies assessing the impact of listening to Indian classical music on the EEG. Primary outcomes were changes in the brain waves, frequency, and power and their relationship to activity-related arousal, attention, and mental tasks. The studies were analyzed according to the PRISMA guidelines.

There were a total of five included studies with 71 participants in the age range of 19-30, and the conditions for the test groups were generally similar except for varying types of rāgas used and time of day. Analysis of the data collected from 71 participants revealed that music interventions had statistically significant effects on increasing alpha activity and attention scores. Fractal analysis was sensitive enough to detect EEG brainwave changes while and after listening to the rāga musical intervention. Rāgas stimulate arousal in different areas of the brain, depending on the emotions they are designed to evoke. However, the synchronized studies together could not highlight a significant relationship between rāgas and EEG fractal dimension values.

Although the meta-analysis failed to reproduce the same results from the individual studies, potentially due to the small sample size and study variation, the meta-analysis opens doors to the potential of rāgas to elicit distinct emotions and serve as robust predictors of emotional response. Future studies can explore the therapeutic potential of various rāgas in the clinical setting, such as in the management of cognitive disorders and stress or in modulating heart rate variability and cognitive performance.

Introduction and background

The bloom and progress of human civilization have always been accompanied and intertwined with music. It is widely acknowledged that music can influence the human body's resonance and trigger a unique emotional response [ 1 ]. Particularly, various properties of a musical piece can either align or conflict with the body’s own rhythms, thereby conferring a distinct effect [ 2 ]. Regardless, the effect of music on the human body is multifarious. Studies have found that in the brain alone, music can help release the ‘happy’ neurotransmitters, modulate emotional states and mood, and reduce the perception of pain [ 3 , 4 ]. Listening to music helps keep the neurons and synapses more active, as different parts of the brain are involved in processing music [ 4 ].

These aspects have improved the acceptance of music-based interventions and music therapy across specialties. In recent years, the focus has moved to explore different types of mindful music and their applications in the clinical setting.

One such form of mindful music that is gaining interest is Indian classical music (ICM), colloquially known as rāgas [ 5 ]. It is one of the oldest systems of music in the world, but it is still practiced today. Literally, the prefix ra refers to the ‘tune of life’, so rāga means the coloring of life [ 5 ]. Rāgas are considered to be a fluid form of music, known for how each musical note has guidelines yet is improvisational at the same time [ 5 ]. This aspect makes the soundscape unique from other forms of music commonly employed in music therapy interventions.

Particularly, both ICM and Western music employ the same notes but differ in their styles (Figure ​ (Figure4) 4 ) [ 5 ]. Rāgas place an emphasis on melody and rhythm and are thus monophonic in nature [ 5 ]. Meanwhile, western music joins many notes together to form chords, making it polyphonic [ 5 ]. Rāgas also have significant droning, or instantaneous acoustic oscillations, which are produced from instruments like the Tanpura or Surpeti. This aspect is collectively known as the Tanpura drone [ 5 ].

Under the ICM genre, there are many families and forms of rāgas, whereby unique rāgas have been ascribed to a certain time of day or season [ 6 ]. For example, Rāg Bhairav is encouraged to be played in the morning [ 6 ]. All of these elements of ICM are designed uniquely for each rāga to evoke a particular emotion and modulate arousal at particular times of the day [ 6 ].

Past neurological studies have also identified that music therapies can be evaluated with electroencephalogram (EEG) studies to explore their impact on the brain system [ 7 ]. Each frequency band of the EEG rhythm relates to specific functions of the brain. For example, the frontal midline theta rhythm (Fm theta) often appears on the EEG during consecutive mental tasks [ 8 ]. The features of ICM have encouraged studies to investigate the impact of listening rāgas on the EEG. A randomized control trial investigating Rāga Desi Todi in healthy college students found a significant increase in alpha band activity and a decrease in anxiety and depression scores [ 9 ].

As the acceptance of music-based interventions and music therapy improves across specialties, the focus has also moved towards exploring different forms of mindful music and discovering their appropriate applications in the clinical setting. Given its different musical approach, ICM continues to garner attention, necessitating a review to assess the impact of listening to this genre of music on the EEG.

The aim of this study was to analyze published prospective case studies in the form of a meta-analysis to assess the impact of listening to rāgas on the EEG of healthy individuals.

Materials and methods

The review followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [ 10 ].

Search Strategy

The literature search was conducted using PsychINFO, PubMed, Google Scholar, and Web of Science electronic databases with criteria such as “EEG” AND “Indian Classical Music OR Rāgas” with dates from 2010 to January 2022. The search criteria were elaborated by employing the PICO tool to search for articles and determine their eligibility (Table ​ (Table1 1 ).

PICODescriptionSearch terms
PopulationHealthy individuals (i.e., no chronic illnesses or neurological/audiological conditions)Healthy Patients
InterventionIndian classical music/RāgasIndian Classical Music OR Rāgas
ComparisonNo music (negative control) 
OutcomePrimary outcome: changes in EEG and EEG fractal dimension analysis secondary outcome: moderating variables such as music preferenceEEG or EEG fractal analysis

Study Eligibility Criteria

The inclusion criteria for the obtained studies were publicly published prospective studies that assessed the impact of Indian classical music/rāgas on the EEG in English (Figure ​ (Figure1). 1 ). Studies that were excluded were studies that did not include ICM/rāgas as an intervention, did not assess the impact of listening to music using the EEG, or were themselves review articles or other gray literature studies. The studies were manually evaluated for their eligibility among the investigators collectively.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is cureus-0015-00000049592-i01.jpg

Data Extraction and Synthesis

Review Manager 5.3 (The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration, Copenhagen) was used to conduct the meta-analysis and organize the data, whereby an author independently performed the data extraction process after the studies were shortlisted. Study metadata such as the year of publication, sample size, gender, effect, and measures was extracted as an addition to the desired EEG-fractal dimension values for the meta-analysis. The EEG-fractal dimensions were evaluated for changes in brain waves, frequency, and power based on activity-related arousals. Study characteristics such as age group, listening time, type of rāga, and time of day were also tabulated to understand the degree of heterogeneity among the studies. Qualitative aspects of the results and additional measures of arousal (i.e., psychometrics) were also noted separately if they were employed in the study but not included in the meta-analysis.

Quality Assessment and Risk of Bias

The quality of the studies and their individual risk of biases were appraised using the Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies (QATQS) [ 11 ]. Two authors independently reviewed the selected studies using the QATQS tool and assigned a global rating to each study.

Eligible Studies

The quality of the studies and their individual risk of biases were appraised using the QATQS [ 11 ]. Two authors independently reviewed the selected studies using the QATQS tool and assigned a global rating to each study.

Across the individual studies, the sample sizes ranged from two to 30, with a total of 71 participants across all five studies. Male participants were dominant in the review, with an average of 59% males across all five studies. Among the five studies included, there were three major musical interventions: Tanpura drone, Hindustani rāga, and Carnatic rāga. One study included two positive control groups of subjects listening to hard rock and jazz. The time-of-day participants received musical intervention, and other relevant baseline characteristics are tabulated (Table ​ (Table2). 2 ). Age distribution was measured differently across the studies (Table ​ (Table2 2 ).

StudyAuthorn% FemaleAge mean ± SD (min-max)InterventionTime of dayGlobal ratingEEG changes
1Banerjee et al. [ ]2120%(20-25)Tanpura DroneAfternoonModerateSignificant changes in EEG-FD
2Sanyal et al. [ ]20%30.0Hindustani RāgaAfternoonWeakIncrease in left frontal lobe arousal after Raga Chhayanat; increase in right frontal lobe arousal after Raga Darbari Kannada.
3Maity et al. [ ]1040%(19-25)Tanpura DroneAfternoonWeakIncrease in alpha and theta spectra width
4Geethanjali et al. [ ]875%20.0±0.4Carnatic RāgaMorningWeakIncrease in beta power
5Kumar et al. [ ]3050%24.8±7.4/27.6±6.4Carnatic RāgaMorningStrongIncreased Alpha-wave frequency 

Results of Individual Studies

Banerjee et al. [ 9 ] assessed the change in brain state when subjected to audio. The investigation employed fractal dimensions, which is a sensitive parameter capable of distinguishing brain states even with an acoustic signal of simple musical structure. The study by Sanyal et al. [ 12 ] analyzed the effect of Hindustani music on brain activity during normal relaxing conditions using EEG. The finding showed that arousal-based activities were enhanced while listening to Hindustani music of contrasting emotions. Maity et al. [ 13 ] study the different levels of neural activation in the human alpha and theta brain rhythms under the effect of simple acoustical stimuli using different EEG feature classification techniques such as wavelet transform (WT) and multifractal detrended fluctuation analysis (MFDFA). It was found that in all the frontal electrodes, alpha as well as theta complexity increases, as is evident from the increase in multifractal spectral width.

Geethanjali et al. [ 14 ] also assessed the effect of music on brain function rate based on the experimenter’s choice of music (jazz, carnatic, and hard rock) with or without mental workload. The results of the study showed that jazz and carnatic music significantly improve brain function as compared to hard rock during mental tasks. Kumar et al. [ 15 ] assessed the effect of Indian classical music on brain activity using EEG. This study found that there is an effect of music on alpha waves.

Five studies report the effect of ancient Indian rāgas using EEG before and after using a random effect model [ 9 , 12 - 15 ].

The meta-analysis indicated that, contrary to the findings of individual studies, there is no statistically significant difference in fractal dimensions observed before and after EEG. Test for overall effect: Z = 0.28 (MD = −0.04, CI: −0.35 to 0.26 (p = 0.78>0.05) (Figure ​ (Figure2). 2 ). The funnel plot (Figure ​ (Figure3) 3 ) suggests that most of the studies chosen had sufficient statistical power for analysis considering the context of what is being investigated.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is cureus-0015-00000049592-i02.jpg

The individual studies were promising in demonstrating a relationship between the music intervention and EEG alpha activity along with attention scores. Fractal analysis was sensitive enough to detect EEG brainwave changes during and after listening to the rāga music intervention. However, the meta-analysis was inconclusive, unable to generate a statistically significant relationship between the music intervention and EEG brainwave changes. The discrepancy can be attributed to the fewer studies and varying study designs employed in each experiment. For example, the duration and time of day subjects received the rāga music intervention differed in every study. Preference or familiarity with a certain form of music could also be a factor in such EEG studies. From the studies, it was unclear whether the participants preferred rāgas or not.

Therefore, future studies can evaluate the impact of other variables, such as listening duration, participant preference, type of rāga, time of day, and specific physical and psychological participant covariates, to better delineate the potential of rāgas. Such variables, especially the time of day, can have significant effects. For example, music with an upbeat tempo can have different effects in the morning hours compared to the evening hours [ 16 ]. Participant preference is also a significant covariate and is often considered when developing guidelines for music-based interventions [ 17 ]. This can be coupled with randomized-controlled trials using particular rāgas lauded for their therapeutic properties as a music intervention in various contexts. The individual studies also did not mention the state of wakefulness among subjects or discuss in detail frontal rhythmic theta waves in adolescent subjects, both factors that are relevant to understanding the impact of the musical interventions.

One particular application of raga music therapy can be potential in comatose patients. The bioelectrical nature of the brain in comatose patients can be characterized by low amounts of high-frequency waves [ 18 ]. In fact, the EEG in comatose patients has prognostic significance: a more suppressed EEG is typically associated with a poorer prognosis, while the return of high-frequency waves may indicate higher chances for recovery [ 19 ].

As rāgas were found by some studies to increase the levels of alpha and beta waves [ 20 ], a study can evaluate the impact of listening to rāgas on the EEG of comatose or traumatic brain injury patients. Rāgas can also be evaluated in psychiatric patients, as one of the studies found that it increased attention scores among subjects, and another background study found a decrease in depression inventory scores. Likewise, music therapy has been previously found beneficial in schizophrenia patients, so a study can evaluate the benefits of listening to rāgas in such contexts [ 21 , 22 ].

Notably, while our meta-analysis specifically focused on the effects of rāgas, there are several similar studies analyzing brainwave changes upon listening to Nasyids (Nasheeds). Nasyids, akin to rāgas, place heavy emphasis on vocal expression but are not accompanied by musical instruments. Particularly interesting are two preliminary studies that paralleled the study by Geethanjali et al., comparing Nasyids to rock music [ 23 ] and Western classical music [ 24 ]. Both studies demonstrated a more pronounced increase in alpha band activity when listening to Nasyids as compared to rock music or Western classical music [ 23 , 24 ]. An additional study not only found comparable impacts on alpha band activity but also revealed that Nasyids have a mild moderating effect on diastolic blood pressure [ 25 ]. These findings can be coupled with our meta-analysis as another avenue of research. Future studies can explore advanced data science techniques, such as machine learning clustering, to identify the distinctive features of specific classes of mindful music and their correlation with brainwave changes [ 26 ]. This approach may provide a more nuanced understanding of the intricacies between sound characteristics and neurophysiological responses.

Rāgas employ unique pitch, time, and dynamics, along with their own spatial and temporal characteristics. This meta-analysis opens doors to the potential of rāgas to elicit distinct emotions and serve as robust predictors of emotional response. Future studies can explore the therapeutic potential of various rāgas in the clinical setting.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the meta-analysis was inconclusive and contradicted the results of the individual studies. The individual studies found that South Asian ragas increase alpha activity and improve attention scores. The likely discrepancy could be attributed to the limited number of investigations involving this topic as well as slight variations across the study designs that could not have been statistically classified. One aspect that must be acknowledged is that there is a limited availability of studies on this topic. Regardless, this systematic review and meta-analysis opens doors to further investigation in the area to better understand the therapeutic potential of integrating various genres of music in the clinical setting.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is cureus-0015-00000049592-i04.jpg

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Author Contributions

Concept and design:   Shrinit Babel, Suman Baral, Abhishek Srivastava

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data:   Shrinit Babel

Drafting of the manuscript:   Shrinit Babel, Abhishek Srivastava

Critical review of the manuscript for important intellectual content:   Shrinit Babel, Suman Baral, Abhishek Srivastava

Supervision:   Suman Baral, Abhishek Srivastava

COMMENTS

  1. Indian Music Research Papers

    Monsoon Feelings: A History of Emotions in the Rain, edited by Imke Rajamani, Margrit Pernau & Katherine Butler Schofield. BOOK: Niyogi Books, New Delhi, 2018. The monsoon is the season of pouring rain and intense emotions: love and longing, hope and fear, pleasure and pain, devotion and joyous excess.

  2. Sangeet Galaxy

    Profile. "Sangeet Galaxy" is a first online Peer-Reviewed Journal dedicated to Indian music. Sangeet Galaxy seeks to publish a balanced mix of high-quality research paper/article from the different domains of Music. 'Sangeet Galaxy' journal is eager to develop a strong co-ordination with researchers, academicians and music readers. Our ...

  3. A Reflection on the Aesthetics of Indian Music, With Special Reference

    It is commonly taken for granted by many students of Indian music, especially those studying Hindustani Raga-Sangita, that the mere singing or playing of the musical notes and the emanating melodies correctly with regard to the technical and grammatical aspects of the art-form are enough to create the aesthetic.This problem has led to two main wrong practices among those learners, namely, (a ...

  4. Music and Emotion—A Case for North Indian Classical Music

    Language Literacy and Music Laboratory, National Brain Research Centre, Manesar, India; The ragas of North Indian Classical Music (NICM) have been historically known to elicit emotions. Recently, Mathur et al. (2015) provided empirical support for these historical assumptions, that distinct ragas elicit distinct emotional responses. In this review, we discuss the findings of Mathur et al ...

  5. Indian Classical Music: Traits and Trends

    The paper 'Indian Classical music in the globalized world' aims at understanding 'globalization' in the context of Indian classical music. ... According to the research, the music types which will ...

  6. Indian Classical Music Research Papers

    Philosophical Nature of Indian Music: This article is about the diversity and the interconnectivity between various forms of classical music spread across the Indian subcontinent. It has been published in the August 2020 issue of Yojana , a monthly journal devoted to the socio-economic issues of India, published by Publication Division, India.

  7. Popular Musics of India: An Ethnomusicological Review

    how popular music culture in India has developed since early 1990s, because of the economic liberalisation and the growing influence of globalisation on the cultural sphere in the country. However, Indian popular music scene as a whole has be en vastly understudied. The history of Indian popular music shares an inextricable nexus with the varying

  8. indian classical music Latest Research Papers

    The ragas of Indian classical music are believed to have therapeutic effects. This study was done to investigate the impact of an Indian raga on mental wellbeing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Materials and Methods: It was a one-group before-after design study. A total of 45 adult subjects of Kolkata, recruited online, using convenience sampling ...

  9. Relevant areas of research in Indian Musicology today

    But I will discuss only one point in this paper, that is of currently relevant thrust areas of research in Indian music. A research paper submitted by Dr K J Kunte on September 19, 2018, for the conference on 'Music Research' organized by SNDT, Pune. 2 Many preceding thinkers have pointed out several thrust areas in Indian musicology.

  10. Raga Recognition in Indian Classical Music Using Deep Learning

    Raga Recognition in Indian Classical Music. using Deep Learning. Devansh P Shah, Nikhil M Jagtap, Prathmesh T T alekar, and Kiran Ga wande. Sardar Patel Institute of T echnology, Mum bai, India ...

  11. PDF Recent Trends in Indian Classical Music

    The paper 'Changing trends in Indian classical music: ... Indian music has evident of huge changes since its provenance. Through the process of evolvement it has accommodated many features which have completely changes its identity and place. ... International Journal of Research in Engineering, IT and Social Sciences, ISSN 2250-0588, Impact ...

  12. A Comparative Study of Audio Source Separation Techniques on Indian

    The research paper thoroughly explores the theoretical foundations of these separation algorithms, their implementation, and their evaluation on Indian classical music. This research contributes to the advancement of audio signal processing techniques, providing valuable tools to manipulate, understand, and extract meaningful information from ...

  13. Impact of Listening to Indian Classical Music, or Rāgas, on the

    Introduction and background. The bloom and progress of human civilization have always been accompanied and intertwined with music. It is widely acknowledged that music can influence the human body's resonance and trigger a unique emotional response [].Particularly, various properties of a musical piece can either align or conflict with the body's own rhythms, thereby conferring a distinct ...

  14. FOLK MUSIC AND INDIAN MUSICOLOGY

    Abstract. Folk Music is best defines as a music which are current in the repertory of folk, primitive and tribal society of countryside of India. It is the module to express the entire sentiments ...

  15. PDF Hindustani Music: An Ocean of Sound-Glance on its History

    Indian music grew slowly but subtly from simple Vedic Scales to a highly advanced art music. The ... Despite this, a number of research demonstrate that music can improve human health and performance. a. Music Improves Memory The impact of music on memory has sparked intense discussion in the scientific community, but new study

  16. The changing shape of the Indian recorded music industry in the age of

    India's recorded music industry has grown rapidly in recent years (IMI 2022b) and has been described as 'the sleeping giant' of global music markets (Hu 2017). In 2020, music streaming revenues accounted for 85.1% of overall recorded music revenues in India (IMI 2022a, 8). Furthermore, in a short span of three years, India has entered the ...

  17. Indian Classical Music, Raaga as music therapy: scope and opportunities

    Listening to ragas can help regulate blood pressure and heart rate and may affect the sympathetic and vagal systems [10]. The research suggests that just 10 minutes of listening to music can lead to an acute reduction in anxiety and improvements in cardiovascular parameters [11]. Different ragas can have varying effects.

  18. (PDF) Indian Classical Music under Digitization

    The present paper tries to understand and analyze the state of Indian Classical Music amongst the web of information technology. The data used herein are the secondary source obtained from various ...

  19. PDF Music: An Integral Element of Indian Theatre Forms

    IJCRT2009340 International Journal of Creative Research Thoughts (IJCRT) www.ijcrt.org 2688 Music: An Integral Element of Indian Theatre Forms Saish Deshpande Research Scholar, Department of Music & Dramatics Shivaji University, Kolhapur Co-author: Prof. Dr. Rajan Gavas Former Head - Department of Marathi Shivaji University, Kolhapur

  20. India's rich musical heritage has a lot to offer to modern psychiatry

    Music with its instantaneous pleasing effect can be an answer to misery. It is a form of art that is easily accessible anytime and anywhere. This article gives an overview of music therapy practiced in ancient India, its influence on emotion and mind, and speculates its possible clinical applications in the modern era based on the available scientific literature.

  21. (PDF) Music Therapy Research In India: A Review

    Additionally, the paper explores the impact of cultural factors on the practice of music therapy in India, including the incorporation of traditional musical elements and indigenous healing practices.