• Our Mission

Explaining the Symbiotic Relationship Between Reading and Writing

Students who understand how reading relates to writing and vice versa can develop into better writers.

Photo of high school students writing

For elementary school teachers, the saying is, students learn to read and then read to learn. At the middle and high school levels, teachers may experience the relationship of first writing to read, and then reading to write. Although this expression is not so common, there are resources that point to such a relationship, including ” Writing to Read: Evidence for How Writing Can Improve Reading ” or books such as The Write to Read .

While the relationship of “writing to read” and “reading to write” represents a symbiotic one, there is a distinct difference that may help us better understand what we are teaching students.

Writing to Read

When a student is writing to read, they are using writing as a tool to truly understand the reading. Writing to read is driven by the text the student is studying.

Examples of writing to read

  • Write a high-level summary to remember and consolidate the content of the reading.
  • Write a claim about the reading, and use three pieces of evidence to support it.
  • Respond to open-ended questions about the reading as a way to connect to or analyze the text.
  • Write an essay on a book to illuminate a particular theme or provide evidence to trace tonal shifts in the piece.
  • Write about the content of a particular text or texts to understand, analyze, or evaluate the text(s) at a deeper level.
  • Annotate during reading to capture important terms, ideas, or content.
  • Fill in graphic organizers or take notes to track the reading.
  • Write in response to a prompt.
  • Write an essay about a particular literary device or critical feature of the text(s).

Reading to Write

When a student is reading to write, they are using reading as a tool to improve their writing. Reading to write occurs when students first learn how to imitate their favorite authors, historians, scientists, or researchers. This is the deliberate use of mentor texts to mold a student’s writing ability.

Examples of reading to write

  • Read memoirs or personal essays to prepare to write a college essay.
  • Read several articles from a particular journal or newspaper to increase knowledge of stylistic expectations in preparation to write a piece for publication.
  • To help relieve writer’s block, use reading to think about different ways to write.
  • Read widely on a topic to consider one’s own writing approach and background knowledge.
  • Review multiple texts to write for a particular purpose or on a specific topic.
  • Read a lot to write more by picking out the ideas that spark thinking.
  • Maintain an annotated bibliography of mentor texts that serve as a writing coach.

Moving Toward Reading to Write

The developmental progression from reader to writer is specific to each student’s experience; however, we do know that in order to strengthen their ability to write, students must continue to read more.

Reading feeds writing. When writing dries up or stalls, the best way to revitalize it is to feed your brain with more reading. Reading may be compared to eating the nutrients we need for the energy to write. Reading feeds the writer with ideas for structure, rich language, literary moves, and compelling ways to illuminate a writer’s purpose.

After filling our brain with reading, turning back to writing typically gives one the energy needed to continue. This is one reason why writing to read is so important early on, then gradually becomes just as important as reading to write. As students develop confidence and competence as readers as the content and vocabulary become much more sophisticated, they build capacity to see the text as both a reader and a writer.

There are potential benefits of looking at the writing-to-read and reading-to-write relationship as teachers continue to challenge themselves with the best way to teach students how to write. Many times at the middle and high school levels, experience with writing to read is the dominant one. If this is the case, it might be a good time to rethink instructional goals and associated assessments.

Here are some practical suggestions for how to weave the two more seamlessly so that students grow into stronger writers.

Assignments that Weave in Reading to Write

After students complete a writing-to-read activity, have them complete a second activity that asks them to use the same text as a reading-to-write activity. (Models and research on how to use mentor texts can be found in books by Allison Marchetti and Rebekah O’Dell .) The second activity may be practice captured in the writer’s notebook for the student to use as a resource to support their writing throughout the year. Some examples of such activities may include the following:

  • Write a high-level summary of the text, then pick sentences from the text that use punctuation or sentence structure in a way that is powerful.
  • Write a claim along with supporting evidence, then look at the text to pick out the best use of transitions.
  • Annotate a book to trace character development, then pull out parts of the book that were written with vivid, descriptive language.
  • Write an essay on the theme of a book, then write a reflection on the author’s craft.
  • Write a response to reading to analyze the author’s line of reasoning, then break down the formal structure of the argument.

After high school, students contribute even more to society, so they need to know how to cogently express their thinking to others. Empowering students as writers requires practice, and it’s important that students understand how writing to read and reading to write serve them in markedly different ways.

4 Ways Reading and Writing Interlock: What the Research Says

relationship between speech reading and writing

  • Share article

In putting together this special report on how writing instruction can and should build on the science of reading, Education Week reporters read through dozens of studies and spoke to leading researchers in the field.

From this reporting, we landed on four main research takeaways, each of which are worth reiterating here and consulting as school districts assess the strength of their own writing programs.

1. Reading and writing are intimately connected.

Research on the connections between the two disciplines began in the early 1980s and has grown more robust with time. Although there are elements specific to each, like handwriting, that need to be practiced on their own, reading and writing instruction appear to be effective when combined.

Among the newest and most important additions are three research syntheses conducted by Steve Graham, a professor at the University of Arizona, and his research partners. One of them examined whether writing instruction also led to improvements in students’ reading ability; a second examined the inverse question. Both found significant positive effects for reading and writing.

A third meta-analysis gets one step closer to classroom instruction. Graham and partners examined 47 studies of instructional programs that balanced both reading and writing —no program could feature more than 60 percent of one or the other. The results showed generally positive effects on both reading and writing measures.

2. Writing matters even at the earliest grades, when students are learning to read.

Studies show that the prewriting students do in early education carries meaningful signals about their decoding, spelling, and reading comprehension later on.

Reading experts say that students should be supported in writing almost as soon as they begin reading, and evidence suggests that both spelling and handwriting are linked to the ability to connect speech to print—a process known as encoding —and to oral language development.

3. Like reading, writing must be taught explicitly.

Writing is a complex task that demands much of students’ cognitive resources. Researchers generally agree that writing must be explicitly taught—rather than left up to students to “figure out” the rules on their own. That way, they can spend more time focusing on what they want to say, rather than trying to determine how to say it effectively.

There isn’t as much research about how precisely to do this. One 2019 review, in fact, found significant overlap among the dozen writing programs studied , and concluded that all showed signs of boosting learning. Debates abound about the amount of structure students need and in what sequence, such as whether they need to master sentence construction before moving onto paragraphs and lengthier texts.

But in general, students should be guided on how to construct sentences and paragraphs, and they should have access to models and exemplars, the research suggests. They also need to understand the iterative nature of writing, including how to draft and revise.

A number of different writing frameworks incorporating various degrees of structure and modeling are available, though most of them have not been studied empirically.

4. Writing can help students learn content—and make sense of it.

Much of reading comprehension depends on helping students absorb “world knowledge”—think arts, ancient cultures, literature, and science—so that they can make sense of increasingly sophisticated texts and ideas as their reading improves. Writing can enhance students’ absorption of this background knowledge, and should be emphasized rather than taking a back seat to the more commonly taught exercises, such as stories and personal reflections.

Graham and colleagues conducted another meta-analysis of nearly 60 studies looking at this idea of “writing to learn” in mathematics, science, and social studies. The studies included a mix of higher-order assignments, like analyses and argumentative writing, and lower-level ones, like summarizing and explaining.

This bibliography is by no means comprehensive, but it includes some of the studies and commentaries that we found most helpful in putting together this special report.

Berninger V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Graham S., & Richards T. (2002). Writing and reading: Connections between language by hand and language by eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities. Special Issue: The Language of Written Language, 35(1), 39–56 Berninger, Virginia, Robert D. Abbott, Janine Jones, Beverly J. Wolf, Laura Gould, Marci Anderson-Younstrom, Shirley Shimada, Kenn Apel. (2006) “Early development of language by hand: composing, reading, listening, and speaking connections; three letter-writing modes; and fast mapping in spelling.” Developmental Neuropsychology, 29(1), pp. 61-92 Cabell, Sonia Q, Laura S. Tortorelli, and Hope K. Gerde (2013). “How Do I Write…? Scaffolding Preschoolers’ Early Writing Skills.” The Reading Teacher, 66(8), pp. 650-659. Gerde, H.K., Bingham, G.E. & Wasik, B.A. (2012). “Writing in Early Childhood Classrooms: Guidance for Best Practices.” Early Childhood Education Journal 40, 351–359 (2012) Gilbert, Jennifer, and Steve Graham. (2010). “Teaching Writing to Elementary Students in Grades 4–6: A National Survey.” The Elementary School Journal 110(44) Graham, Steve, et al. (2017). “Effectiveness of Literacy Programs Balancing Reading and Writing Instruction: A Meta-Analysis.” Reading Research Quarterly, 53(3) pp. 279–304 Graham, Steve, and Michael Hebert. (2011). “Writing to Read: A Meta-Analysis of the Impact of Writing and Writing Instruction on Reading.” Harvard Educational Review (2011) 81(4): 710–744. Graham, Steve. (2020). “The Sciences of Reading and Writing Must Become More Fully Integrated.” Reading Research Quarterly, 55(S1) pp. S35–S44 Graham, Steve, Sharlene A. Kiuhara, and Meade MacKay. (2020).”The Effects of Writing on Learning in Science, Social Studies, and Mathematics: A Meta-Analysis.” Review of Educational Research April 2020, Vol 90, No. 2, pp. 179–226 Shanahan, Timothy. “History of Writing and Reading Connections.” in Shanahan, Timothy. (2016). “Relationships between reading and writing development.” In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (2nd ed., pp. 194–207). New York, NY: Guilford. Slavin, Robert, Lake, C., Inns, A., Baye, A., Dachet, D., & Haslam, J. (2019). “A quantitative synthesis of research on writing approaches in grades 2 to 12.” London: Education Endowment Foundation. Troia, Gary. (2014). Evidence-based practices for writing instruction (Document No. IC-5). Retrieved from University of Florida, Collaboration for Effective Educator, Development, Accountability, and Reform Center website: http://ceedar.education.ufl.edu/tools/innovation-configuration/ Troia, Gary, and Steve Graham. (2016).“Common Core Writing and Language Standards and Aligned State Assessments: A National Survey of Teacher Beliefs and Attitudes.” Reading and Writing 29(9).

A version of this article appeared in the January 25, 2023 edition of Education Week as 4 Key Things to Know About How Reading and Writing Interlock

Sign Up for EdWeek Update

Edweek top school jobs.

Group of kids reading while sitting on the floor in the library

Sign Up & Sign In

module image 9

On the relationship between speech and writing with implications for behavioral approaches to teaching literacy

  • Published: 08 July 2017
  • Volume 8 , pages 127–140, ( 1990 )

Cite this article

relationship between speech reading and writing

  • Roy A. Moxley 1  

Two theories of the relationship between speech and writing are examined. One theory holds that writing is restricted to a one-way relationship with speech—a unidirectional influence from speech to writing. In this theory, writing is derived from speech and is simply a representation of speech. The other theory holds that additional, multidirectional influences are involved in the development of writing. The unidirectional theory focuses on correspondences between speech and writing while the multidirectional theory directs attention to the differences as well as the similarities between speech and writing. These theories have distinctive pedagogical implications. Although early behaviorism may be seen to have offered some support for the unidirectional theory, modern behavior analysis should be seen to support the multidirectional theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

relationship between speech reading and writing

Rewriting the Book: New Literacy Practices and Their Implications for Teaching and Evaluating Writing

relationship between speech reading and writing

Genres and Institutions: Functional Perspectives on Educational Discourse

relationship between speech reading and writing

Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar  

Anderson, R. C., Hiebart, E. F., Scott, J. A., & Wilkinson, I. A. (1985). Emerging literacy. In Becoming a nation of readers (pp. 21–58). Washington, DC: The National Institute of Education.

Aristotle (1938). On interpretation (H. P. Cook, trans.). In The Organon (Vol. 1, pp. 115–179). London: Heinemann.

Bailey, J. S. (1987). On having an impact. The ABA Newsletter , 10 (2).

Becker, W. (1977). Teaching reading and language to the disadvantaged—What we have learned from field research. Harvard Educational Review, 47 , 518–543.

Article   Google Scholar  

Bereiter, C. (1986). Does direct instruction cause delinquency? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 1 , 289–292.

Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Bloomfield, L. (1942). Linguistics and reading. Elementary English Review, 19 , 125–130, 183–186.

Bloomfield, L., & Barnhart, C. (1961). Let’s read. Detroit: Wayne State U. Press.

Carbo, M. (1988). Debunking the great phonics myth. Phi Delta Kappan, 70 , 226–240.

Chall, J. S. (1989). Learning to read: The great debate 20 years later—A response to ‘debunking the great phonics myth.’ Phi Delta Kappan, 70 , 521–538.

Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of B. F. Skinner, Verbal Behavior (1957). Language, 35 , 26–58.

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.

Clanchy, M. T. (1979). From memory to written record. Cambridge, MA: Harvard U. Press.

Cudd, E. T., & Roberts, L. L. (1986). Using story frames to develop reading comprehension in a 1st grade classroom, The Reading Teacher, 40 , pp. 74–79.

de Saussure, F. (1966). Course in general linguistics , (W. Baskin, Trans.). New York: McGraw-Hill. (Original work published 1915).

Durkin, D. (1966). Children who read early. New York: Teachers College Press.

Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1986). The influence of spelling on speech. In D. Yaden and S. Templeton (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness and beginning literacy (pp. 101–114). Exeter, NH: Heinemann.

Eisenstein, E. L. (1983). The printing revolution in early modern Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge U. Press.

Engelmann, S., Haddox, P., & Bruner, E. (1983). Teach your child to read in 100 easy lessons. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Esper, E. A. (1968). Mentalism and objectivism in linguistics: The sources of Leonard Bloomfield’s psychology of language. New York: American Elsevier.

Friberg, J. (1984). Numbers and measures in the earliest written records. Scientific American, 250 (2), 110–114, 116–118.

Gelb, I. J. (1963). A study of writing (rev. ed). Chicago: U. of Chicago Press.

Goldiamond, I. (1977). Literary behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10 , 527–529.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Goody, J., & Watt, I. (1968). The consequences of literacy. In J. Goody (Ed.), Literacy in traditional societies (pp. 27–68). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Graves, D. H. (1985). All children can write. Learning Disabilities Focus , 1, 36–43.

Halliday, M. A. K. (1987). Spoken and written modes of meaning. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Comprehending oral and written language (pp. 55–82). New York: Academic Press.

Harris, R. (1986). The origin of writing. La Salle, IL: Open Court.

Havelock, E. A. (1986). The muse learns to write. New Haven, CN: Yale U. Press.

Hewes, G. W. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origin of speech. Current Anthropology, 14 , 5–24.

Holland, J. G. (1979). Analysis of behavior in reading instruction. In L. R. Resnick & P. A. Weaver (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (Vol. 1, pp. 261–277). Hillsdale, NY: Erlbaum.

Householder, F. W. (1971). The primacy of writing. In his Linguistic speculations (pp. 244–264). London: Cambridge U. Press.

Hummel, J. W. (1985). Courseware Revisited: Cloze plus: A context analysis program. Journal of Learning Disabilities , 18, 364–365.

Hymes, D., & Fought, J. (1981). American structuralism. The Hague: Mouton.

Ifrah, G. (1987). From one to zero: A universal history of numbers. New York: Penguin.

Jasim, S. A., & Oates, J. (1986). Early tokens and tablets in Mesopotamia. World Archaeology, 17 , 348–362.

Johnston, V. M. (1985). Introducing the microcomputer into English. III. An evaluation of TRAY as a program using problem-solving as a strategy for developing reading skills. British Journal of Educational Technology, 16 (3), 208–218.

Julia, P. (1983). Explanatory models in linguistics: A behavioral perspective. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Book   Google Scholar  

Kantor, J. R. (1936). An objective psychology of grammar. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Publications, Science Series No. 1.

Kantor, J. R. (1977). Psychological linguistics. Chicago: Principia Press.

Kolers, P. A. (1985). Phonology in reading. In D. R. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning (pp. 404–411). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Lehr, F. (1987). ERIC/RCS: Story Grammar. The Reading Teacher, 40 , 550–552.

LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1985). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. In H. Singer & R. B. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (pp. 689–718). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Moffett, J., & Wagner, B. J. (1983). Student-centered language arts and reading, k-13 (3rd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Moran, M. R. (1987). Epilogue. Topics in language disorders, 7 , 86–89.

Morrow, L. M., & Weinstein, C. S. (1982). Increasing children’s use of literature through program and physical design changes. The Elementary School Journal, 83 , 131–137.

Moxley, R. A. (1982). Writing and reading in early childhood: A functional approach. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technology.

Moxley, R. A. (1986). A functional analysis of reading. In P. N. Chase & L. J. Parrott (Eds.), Psychological aspects of language (pp. 209–232). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.

Moxley, R. A. (1990). A review of Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Educational Technology, 30 , (10), 56–57.

Moxley, R. A., & Joyce, B. G. (1990). Early visual spelling on the microcomputer. Journal of Computing in Childhood Educaton, 1 (3), 3–14.

Nystrand, M. (1987). The role of context in communication. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Comprehending oral and written language (pp. 197–214). New York: Academic Press.

Olson, D. R., Torrance, N., & Hildyard, A. (Eds.). (1985). Literacy, language, and learning. Cambridge: Cambridge U. Press.

Ong, W. J. (1982). Orality and literacy. New York: Methuen.

Otto, J. (1982). The new debate in reading. The Reading Teacher, 36 , 14–18.

Peirce, C. S. (1960–1961). Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce (C. Hartshorne and P. Weiss [Eds.] Vols. 1–4). Cambridge, MA: Belknap.

Pike, K. L. (1947). Phonemics: A technique for reducing languages to writing. University of Michigan Publications, Linguistics 3. Ann Arbor. MI: University of Michigan Press.

Powell, R. P., & Still, A. W. (1979). Behaviorism and the psychology of language. Behaviorism, 7 , 71–89.

Price, E. H. (1976). How thirty-seven gifted children learned to read. The Reading Teacher, 30 , 44–48.

Sapir, E. (1921). Language. New York: Harcourt, Brace.

Schmandt-Besserat, D. (1978). The earliest precursor of writing. Scientific American, 238 (6), 50–59.

Schweinhart, L. J., Weikart, D. P., & Larner, M. B. (1986). Consequences of three preschool curriculum models. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 1 , 15–45.

Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Skinner, B. F. (1972). Why do we need teaching machines. In his Cumulative record (3rd ed., pp. 171–193) New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Skinner, B. F. (1989). Recent issues in the analysis of behavior. Columbus, OH: Merrill.

Smith, F. (1973). Decoding: The great fallacy. In his Psycholinguistics and reading (pp. 70–83). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Smith, F. (1989). Understanding reading (4th ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Staats, A. W. (1968). Learning, language, and cognition. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Steinberg, D. D., & Steinberg, M. T. (1975). Reading before speaking. Visible Language , 9, 197–224.

Stotsky, S. (1983). Research on reading/writing relationships. Language Arts, 60 , 627–642.

Stotsky, S. (1987). A comparison of the two theories about development in written language: Implications for pedagogy and research. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Comprehending oral and written language (pp. 371–395). New York: Academic Press.

Street, B. V. (1984). Literacy in theory and practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Stubbs, M. (1980). Language and literacy. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Stubbs, M. (1986). Educational linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell.

Sulzby, E. (1986). Writing and reading: Signs of oral and written language organization in the young child. In W. H. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Emergent literacy: Writing and reading (pp. 50–89). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Taylor, N. E., Blum, I. H., & Logsdon, D. M. (1986). The development of written language awareness: Environmental aspects and program characteristics. Reading Research Quarterly, 21 , 132–149.

Torrey, J. W. (1973). Learning to read without a teacher: A case study. In F. Smith (Ed.), Psycholinguistics and reading (pp. 147–157). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Tweney, R. D. (1979). Reflections on the history of behavioral theories of language. Behaviorism, 7 , 91–103.

Yates, F. A. (1966). The art of memory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

West Virginia University, 604 Allen Hall, Morgantown, WV, 26506-6122, USA

Roy A. Moxley

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Moxley, R.A. On the relationship between speech and writing with implications for behavioral approaches to teaching literacy. Analysis Verbal Behav 8 , 127–140 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392853

Download citation

Published : 08 July 2017

Issue Date : April 1990

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392853

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

On the relationship between speech and writing with implications for behavioral approaches to teaching literacy

  • PMID: 22477610
  • PMCID: PMC2748615
  • DOI: 10.1007/BF03392853

Two theories of the relationship between speech and writing are examined. One theory holds that writing is restricted to a one-way relationship with speech-a unidirectional influence from speech to writing. In this theory, writing is derived from speech and is simply a representation of speech. The other theory holds that additional, multidirectional influences are involved in the development of writing. The unidirectional theory focuses on correspondences between speech and writing while the multidirectional theory directs attention to the differences as well as the similarities between speech and writing. These theories have distinctive pedagogical implications. Although early behaviorism may be seen to have offered some support for the unidirectional theory, modern behavior analysis should be seen to support the multidirectional theory.

education summary logo

Relationship and Difference Between Speech and Writing in Linguistics

Back to: Pedagogy of English- Unit 4

Many differences exist between the written language and the spoken language. These differences impact subtitling which is a practice that has become highly prevalent in the modern age. It is a process used to translate what the speaker is saying for those of other languages or who are deaf.

The main difference between written and spoken languages is that written language is comparatively more formal and complex than spoken language. Some other differences between the two are as follows:

Relationship and Difference Between Speech and Writing in Linguistics

Writing is more permanent than the spoken word and is changed less easily. Once something is printed, or published on the internet, it is out there for the world to see permanently. In terms of speaking, this permanency is present only if the speaker is recorded but they can restate their position.

Apart from formal speeches, spoken language needs to be produced instantly. Due to this, the spoken word often includes repetitions, interruptions, and incomplete sentences. As a result, writing is more polished.

Punctuation

Written language is more complex than spoken language and requires punctuation. Punctuation has no equivalent in spoken language.

Speakers can receive immediate feedback and can clarify or answer questions as needed but writers can’t receive immediate feedback to know whether their message is understood or not apart from text messages, computer chats, or similar technology.

Writing is used to communicate across time and space for as long as the medium exists and that particular language is understood whereas speech is more immediate.

Use of Slang

Written and spoken communication uses different types of language. For instance, slang and tags are more often used when speaking rather than writing.

Speaking and listening skills are more prevalent in spoken language whereas writing and reading skills are more prevalent in written language.

Tone and pitch are often used in spoken language to improve understanding whereas, in written language, only layout and punctuation are used.

These are the major differences between spoken language and written language.

follow on google news

Reading Worksheets, Spelling, Grammar, Comprehension, Lesson Plans

The Relationship Between Reading and Writing

Reading Writing Puzzle Pieces

F or many years reading and writing were (and sometimes still are) taught separately. Though the two have almost always been taught by the same person (the English/Language Arts teacher) during the Language Arts period or block, educators rarely made explicit connections between the two for their students. Over the last ten years research has shown that reading and writing are more interdependent than we thought. The relationship between reading and writing is a bit like that of the chicken and egg. Which came first is not as important as the fact that without one the other cannot exist. A child’s literacy development is dependent on this interconnection between reading and writing.

Basically put: reading affects writing and writing affects reading. According to recommendations from the major English/Language Arts professional organizations, reading instruction is most effective when intertwined with writing instruction and vice versa. Research has found that when children read extensively they become better writers. Reading a variety of genres helps children learn text structures and language that they can then transfer to their own writing. In addition, reading provides young people with prior knowledge that they can use in their stories. One of the primary reasons that we read is to learn. Especially while we are still in school, a major portion of what we know comes from the texts we read. Since writing is the act of transmitting knowledge in print, we must have information to share before we can write it. Therefore reading plays a major role in writing.

At the same time practice in writing helps children build their reading skills. This is especially true for younger children who are working to develop phonemic awareness and phonics skills. Phonemic awareness (the understanding that words are developed from sound “chunks”) develops as children read and write new words. Similarly, phonics skills or the ability to link sounds together to construct words are reinforced when children read and write the same words. For older children practice in the process of writing their own texts helps them analyze the pieces that they read. They can apply their knowledge about the ways that they chose to use particular language, text structure or content to better understand a professional author’s construction of his or her texts.

Harnessing the Reading-Writing Relationship to Help Children Learn

Simply knowing that reading and writing are intimately connected processes isn’t enough. In order to help children develop these two essential skills, parents and teachers need to apply this knowledge when working with them. Here are a few strategies for using reading and writing to reinforce development of literacy skills.

Genre Study

One of the most effective ways to use the relationship between reading and writing to foster literacy development is by immersing children in a specific genre. Parents and teachers should identify a genre that is essential to a grade level’s curriculum or is of particular interest to a child or group of children. They should then study this genre with the child(ren) from the reading and writing perspectives. Children should read and discuss with adults high quality examples of works written in the genre focusing on its structure and language as well as other basic reading skills including phonics and comprehension. Once children have studied the genre to identify its essential elements, they should be given opportunities to write in the genre. As they are writing, adults should help them apply what they have learned from reading genre specific texts to guide their composition. This process should be recursive to allow children to repeatedly move between reading and writing in the genre. In the end children will not only have a solid and rich knowledge of the genre, but will also have strengthened their general reading and writing skills.

Reading to Develop Specific Writing Skills

Parents and teachers do not have to engage in an extensive genre study to foster their children’s reading and writing abilities. Texts can be used on limited basis to help children learn and strengthen specific writing skills. Parents and teachers should first identify writing skills that a particular child or group of children need support in developing. For example, many students in a seventh grade class might have difficulty writing attention getting introductions in their essays. One of the most effective ways to help children build specific writing skills is to show and discuss with them models that successfully demonstrate the skill. Adults should select a number of texts where the authors “nail” the area that they want to help their children grow in. For our sample seventh graders we’d want to find several pieces of writing with strong, engaging introductions and read and analyze these with the students. Once children have explored effective models of the skill, they should be given opportunities to practice it. They can either write new pieces or revise previous pieces of writing emulating the authors’ techniques.

Integrating “Sound” Instruction in Reading and Writing

Phonemic awareness and phonics are two of the pillars of reading. Without understanding the connection between sounds and letters, a person cannot read. The connection between reading and writing can help solidify these skills in young readers. Parents and teachers should help children “sound out” words in both their reading and writing. When a child comes to a word in their reading that is unfamiliar, the adult(s) working with her can model or guide her in sounding out the word using knowledge of phonemes (sound “chunks”). Similarly, if a child wants to write a new word the adult(s) can use the same technique to help her choose which letters to write. If the child is younger, accurate spelling is not as important as an understanding of the connection between particular sounds and letters. Therefore helping the child pick letters that approximate the spelling is more appropriate than providing him with the actual spelling. If the child is older and has an understanding of some of the unique variations in the English language (such as silent “e”), the parent or teacher should encourage him to use that knowledge to come up with the spelling of the word.

Choice in Reading and Writing

Another effective method for using the relationship between reading and writing to foster literacy development is simply giving children the choice in their reading and writing experiences. We learn best when we are motivated. If children are always told exactly what to read and what to write, they will eventually either come to see reading and writing as impersonal events or will “shut down”. Often in classrooms, teachers allow children to select their own books to read during independent reading time, but they rarely give them the opportunity to pick their own writing topics. In order to encourage ownership over their reading and writing, children should be given chances to read and write what is interesting and important to them.

Talk About It!

While it may seem like common sense to adults that reading and writing have a lot to do with each other, the connection is not always as apparent to young people. Parents and teachers should explain how the two skills reinforce and strengthen each other. Young people (especially adolescents) often ask their parents and teachers, “Why do I have to learn this?” Here is a perfect opportunity to show the relationship between two essential academic and life skills.

  • About Apraxia Kids
  • Walk for Apraxia
  • Find Speech Therapist

Logo

27 Nov Children with Apraxia and Reading, Writing, and Spelling Difficulties

Children with apraxia and reading, writing, and spelling difficulties.

It is not the case that all children with a history of speech and language difficulties have associated literacy problems. However, school-age children whose speech difficulties persist beyond 5 years of age are most at risk for associated difficulties in reading, spelling and sometimes maths. Let us first consider the nature of persisting speech difficulties with reference to a simple psycholinguistic model of speech processing.

This illustrates that we receive spoken information through the ear (input). The information is then processed as it goes up the left hand side of the model and is stored at the top in a word store (lexical representations). When we want to speak we can access stored information and programme it for speaking on the right hand side of the model (output). Some children with speech difficulties have difficulties with speech input (e.g. differentiating between similar sounding words); others have imprecise or ‘fuzzy’ storage of words which makes it difficult to access them (as in word finding difficulties) or to programme a clear production of them because of missing elements in the word store; while others have a difficulty pronouncing speech at an articulatory output level (on the right hand side of the model) even though they know the words involved perfectly well. Children with persisting difficulties, however, may well have pervasive problems which involve all of these aspects of speech processing: input, representations and output. Where this is the case they may also have language difficulties (comprehension and/or _expression). The speech processing system, as illustrated above, is not only the basis for speech and language development but also the foundation for literacy development; ‘written language’ being an extension of ‘spoken language’. For example, if a child has delayed understanding of spoken language s/he will find it very hard to access meaning from the printed word even though s/he may be able to decode the letters perfectly well. Sometimes, children with comprehension or ‘semantic-pragmatic’ difficulties are described as ‘hyperlexic’; this term indicates that a child can read print mechanically better than they can understand it. Other children, particularly those with persisting speech difficulties, have a problem with the mechanics of reading and are more likely to be described as ‘dyslexic’ or as having ‘specific’ reading and spelling problems. This suggests a problem at one or more levels in the speech processing system depicted above.

Typically developing children use this speech processing system not only to develop speech but also use their speech skills to develop another skill: ‘phonological awareness’. This is ‘an ability to reflect on and manipulate the structure of an utterance as distinct from its meaning’. You use your phonological awareness skills to play sound and rhyme games, e.g. judging if two spoken words begin with the same sound or not (e.g. CAT CAR; CAT BALL); or producing a string of words which rhyme with e.g. CAT. Children who find such games difficult, compared to their peers, often have problems with cracking the alphabetic code of languages such as English. Cracking the code is what children do when they sound out letters of a written word and then blend them together to read/pronounce it, or when spelling they take a word, break it up into it into its bits and put letters to each sound segment. Cracking the code therefore involves not just knowing about letters and sounds but also recognising the sequence of sounds in a word, e.g. what is at the beginning, middle and end. This phonological awareness is helped by being able to repeat words consistently and accurately to allow reflection on the structure of the word. Thus, children with persisting speech difficulties often need specific help not only with learning letter sounds and names but also with how these are combined in words through graded phonological awareness activities.

Clear and consistent speech production is particularly important for spelling or when learning new vocabulary. Typically, when asked how many syllables there are in a word (another phonological awareness skill), children repeat it, segment it out loud or in a whisper and then count the beats on their fingers. If they are not able to produce the right number of syllables in the word or if they cannot say the word in the same way on more than one occasion then they cannot spell it correctly or store it clearly. When trying to spell a long word, Danny, a 12 year old boy with apraxia of speech and dyslexic difficulties said exasperatedly: “If I can’t say it I can’t split it up!”

This is a really important insight and true of many children with speech difficulties. Michael, for example, had dyspraxia of speech with inconsistent production of multisyllabic words and particular difficulties producing clusters/blends (e.g. ‘br’ in BRUSH, ‘fl’ in FLOWER, ‘spr’ in SPRAY). His IQ was within normal limits but he had specific reading and spelling difficulties. When trying to spell a long word at 11 years of age he attempted to segment it into its sounds but then transcribed each of his many attempts. The result was rather dramatic. He spelt UMBRELLA as ‘rberherrelrarlsrllles’, and CIGARETTE as ‘satersatarhaelerar’. In his spelling of UMBRELLA he has dropped the first unstressed syllable (‘um’) from his spelling and is trying to write the first stressed syllable ‘br’ which he cannot pronounce. This takes up at least half of the spelling attempt. He is, however, aware that the word includes more than one letter ‘l’! When spelling CIGARETTE he wrote down the beginning sound (‘sa’) and end sound (‘ter’) of the word twice before losing it completely (haelerar). Combining work on all aspects of his speech processing system with phonological awareness and letter knowledge training helped him to have a more consistent approach to his spelling.

Spelling can also be a persisting problem for children who appear to have resolved their speech difficulties .In a recent study we compared the performance of a group of 7 year old children with speech difficulties with a matched control group of their peers (who did not have speech difficulties )on National (UK) tests of reading, spelling and maths. We then compared performance on the same tests of children with persisting speech difficulties with those children who had resolved their speech difficulties. More children with speech difficulties scored below average performance on the tests than did their IQ matched controls, particularly in spelling and reading comprehension. The children who had resolved their speech difficulties performed significantly better than the children who had persisting speech difficulties on all tests and did as well as the controls on everything except spelling.

In summary, children’s speech difficulties arise from problems at one or more points in their underlying speech processing system. This system is the foundation for their written language as well as their spoken language skills. If this foundation is unstable, additional support will be needed to enable a child to use the strengths s/he has to develop phonological awareness skill and letter knowledge. This is tough but not unsurmountable. Once at school, children with delayed spoken and written language can benefit from intensive and explicit letter-sound linkage work coupled where necessary with targeted speech and language work. Add supportive home and school environments and the active involvement of the child in his or her own intervention programme to this and progress will follow. When Danny was asked at 14 years of age what advice he would give to others, he stated:

“If you have any problems to see a therapist, to always try and write letters. Enjoy it. Do not take it as thing you never get out of it ‘cause if you try you will.”

[Professor Joy Stackhouse is a registered speech and language therapist, chartered psychologist and teacher of children with specific literacy difficulties. She currently has the Chair in Human Communication Sciences in the Department of Human Communication Sciences at the University of Sheffield, UK. Prior to this she was Professor of Speech and Literacy at University College London. Joy’s research and practice focuses on children with persisting speech difficulties and their associated literacy and psychosocial development. She has co-authored books and papers in this area particularly with Professor Maggie Snowling, Professor Bill Wells and is currently writing a book on persisting speech difficulties with Dr Michelle Pascoe.]

© Apraxia-KIDS℠ – A program of The Childhood Apraxia of Speech Association (Apraxia Kids) www.apraxia-kids.org

Literacy and Children with Apraxia of Speech

Many parents wonder if their young child with apraxia of speech ( verbal dyspraxia ) will go on to experience difficulties in their education . While there is no certainty that literacy problems will or will not develop, there is research that has shown that children with spoken language problems are at higher risk for literacy related problems . The purpose of this paper is to summarize some pertinent research and articles on the relationship of spoken language problems to literacy development.

Children with spoken language problems may have difficulty developing what are called phonological awareness skills . Joy Stackhouse has described phonological awareness in this way:

“Phonological awareness refers to the ability to reflect on and manipulate the structure of an utterance (e.g., into words, syllables, or sounds) as distinct from its meaning. Children need to develop this awareness to make sense of an alphabetic script, such as English, when learning to read and spell. For example, children have to learn that the sounds ( phonemes ) in a word can be represented by letters ( graphemes ). When spelling a new word, children have to be able to segment the word into its sounds before they can attach the appropriate letters , and when reading an unfamiliar word, they have to be able to decode the printed letters back to sounds .” (Stackhouse, 1997, p.157)

Phonological awareness is made up of many related skills including: recognition and production of rhyme; identification of number of syllables; sound to word matching, word to word matching; sound deletion; and sound segmentation.

Research has found that a strong predictor of literacy development is phonological awareness. Perhaps the stronger predictors of literacy development are later developing phonological awareness skills like sound segmentation and manipulation. When children demonstrate difficulty with phonological awareness, as do many children with spoken language problems, they are at higher risk of difficulty in literacy related skills like reading and spelling. Stackhouse writes that, “Although recent work has clarified how visual deficits may also affect reading performance, there is an overwhelming consensus that verbal skills are the most influential in literacy development (Catts, Hu, Larrivee, & Swank, 1994).” (Stackhouse, 1997, p. 163)

However, not all children with spoken language problems differ in developing reading and spelling skills. Stackhouse compared the reading and spelling skills of a group of children ages 7 – 11 years. She compared children who had speech problems deriving from cleft palate/cleft lip to those described by their therapists as having developmental verbal dyspraxia. The study showed that children with cleft palate did not differ significantly from age-matched typically developing children in tests of reading and spelling. However, the children with developmental verbal dyspraxia did show significant differences and did poorer on these same tests than did their age matched typically developing peers . Many of these children demonstrated errors that suggested they used guess work versus sounding out strategies. Their spelling errors were somewhat bizarre and illogical compared to those children with cleft palate. Therefore, Stackhouse concluded that, ” It is the children with persisting speech difficulties with no obvious medical etiology (who are often described as having phonological impairments or Developmental Verbal Dyspraxia), however, who are most at risk for related specific literacy problems.” (Stackhouse, 1997, p. 169) Other studies appear to report similar findings. One by Bridgeman and Snowling which compared children with developmental verbal dyspraxia to reading age-matched, typically developing children concluded that, “children with persisting phonological impairments have sound segmentation difficulties when processing sound sequences within novel words.” (Stackhouse, 1997, p.175)

A study of four dyspraxic children by Snowling and Stackhouse found that, “children diagnosed as having DVD experience more difficulty in using a phonetic spelling strategy than children who have normal articulation.” (Snowling & Stackhouse, 1983, p. 435) The results support earlier work suggesting that dyspraxic children are less able to carry out grapheme-phoneme conversions than would be predicted from their reading age (Stackhouse, 1982). Finally, Stackhouse writes that, “Persisting phonological impairments beyond the age of 5.6 years may be a sign that a child is at risk for literacy problems.” (Stackhouse, 1997, p.169)

Reading and Spelling Issues:

While many people want to think about reading and spelling as two sides of the same coin, research does not bear this out. Various researchers point to the growing evidence that reading and spelling are independent of one another. For instance, Bradley and Bryant demonstrated that young children may be able to read words that they cannot spell and, conversely, spell words that they cannot read. Snowling and Stackhouse write:

“Spelling is more difficult than reading for most people because reading is a recognition process which can proceed using only ‘partial cues,’ where as spelling is a retrieval process which requires ‘full cues’. In order to spell well one must be able to reproduce the correct letter-by-letter sequence of words (Frith & Frith 1980).” (Snowling & Stackhouse, 1983, p.431)

There are various models that describe the acquisition of reading skills. To simplify this, one can say that in normal reading development children first develop a sight vocabulary – words that they can identify purely by looking at the whole word. This can be described as the direct route . Later, children learn phoneme – grapheme correspondences (sound-letter correspondences) and learn strategies for sounding out words they are trying to read. This can be described as the indirect route . It is important for children to develop this indirect route because if they do not, their reading only progresses to the limits of their visual memory. If they don’t learn phonological strategies via the indirect route then when they are attempting to read an unfamiliar word they will have difficulty decoding the printed letters back to sounds. It is suspected that some children with apraxia of speech or verbal dyspraxia may have difficulty making the leap from the direct to indirect route in reading acquisition.

Spelling Issues

When spelling new words a child needs to be able to segment the word into sounds before they attach an appropriate letter to the sound. In the case of children with apraxia of speech or phonological impairments the types of spelling errors they make aren’t always directly related to their mispronunciation of the words. It is believed that more frequently, their spelling errors are a result of limited phonological awareness, specifically sound segmentation abilities. The literature reports that these children appear to use guesswork vs. logical strategies and that their spelling errors can seem quite bizarre. However, the nature or underlying reason for their spelling difficulties needs to be explored and evaluated because not all children are affected similarly. For any particular child the root of the problem can stem from input, output, representation or a combination of factors.

Some clues that a child with apraxia of speech ( verbal dyspraxia ) may be having difficulty in reading and/or spelling are:

  • The child is not progressing from reading words as visual wholes to breaking the words down into their sounds .
  • The child fails to segment the word into syllables and syllables into sounds . Spelling attempts may seem bizarre.
  • The child has difficulty in rhyme detection and particularly, rhyme production.
  • The child has difficulty with sound blending .

What Can Be Done To Help?

Various studies conducted with children with limited phonological awareness or poor reading skills point to some suggestions. For instance, a study by Hatcher, Hulme, & Ellis who divided seven year olds who were poor readers into three groups and provided different arrangements of phonological awareness training, reading instruction, or no other training outside of the routine classroom work, found that the only group that made significantly more progress than the control group, was the group that focused on both phonological awareness plus explicit reading strategies . In her article reporting the results of this study, Stackhouse writes, “phonological awareness training alone does not necessarily facilitate literacy development. Literacy development is dependent on children’s ability to link their phonological awareness skills to letter knowledge and reading experience.” (Stackhouse, 1997, p.162) An earlier study by Bradley and Bryant of 65 children with below average ability on phonological awareness tasks as nonreaders before entering school demonstrated that phonological awareness training needs to be combined with explicit letter knowledge teaching for these children to make actual gains in literacy development.

Further, a complete language assessment needs to be done. Such an assessment must not only look at and define the symptoms of the speech, reading, and spelling problems but also the underlying nature of those problems. A full assessment would include: speech, language and oral-motor abilities; auditory skills (such as auditory discrimination, memory and organization); rhyme detection and production; syllable and phoneme segmentation; reading comprehension and expression; spelling and awareness of reading and spelling strategies. It is also important to include more difficult items in reading and spelling tests (for instance, multisyllabic words) in order to determine a particular child’s difficulty. At the end of a thorough assessment, Stackhouse suggests that:

“…having identified through the assessment (a) a profile on speech, language, and reading tests (b) the level and modality of breakdown (c) the severity of the difficulties and their manifestation in ‘real life’ (d) the coping strategies adopted then remediation can be planned” (Stackhouse, 1985, p.109)

The Role of the Speech and Language Pathologist:

The literature and studies reviewed for this paper indicate that there needs to be awareness and vigilance to the literacy development of children with spoken language problems, especially those who have apraxia of speech. “There is a danger that as intelligibility reaches an acceptable level, the child is discharged from the speech therapist’s care only to be left struggling with residual speech difficulties and related spelling problems,” concludes Joy Stackhouse. (Stackhouse, 1985, p.115) While the role of the speech and language pathologist is not to teach reading and spelling per se, Snowling & Stackhouse indicate that, “the role is one of identification and promoting the underlying skills that contribute to literacy development .” (Stackhouse, 1997, p.190) It is hoped that by receiving early, intensive communication therapy for apraxia of speech or phonological deficits, these children may, in fact, heighten their phonological awareness and, in part, strengthen a potentially intrinsic weakness.

Stackhouse suggests that some possibly relevant tools, techniques and activities include:

  • phoneme-grapheme matched cards (cards with pictures that represent sounds)
  • color coded systems as visual reminders of language structures or of sound groups
  • sound categorization activities using multi-sensory approaches
  • syllable and sound segmentation activities
  • rhyming work
  • explicit teaching of reading and spelling rules

Reading instruction

Research accumulated over time indicates that many children need explicit teaching using a phonics approach with phonological awareness; sound-letter correspondence and decodable text with kindergartners and first graders. Research appears to indicate that whole language can enhance comprehension and that a balance of comprehension and decoding skills should be focused on but that whole language should not be the only strategy used with nonreaders. This data may be especially important to children with apraxia of speech and residual problems.

What can parents do?

Parents can support the work of speech pathologists and teachers by following through on home activities that are suggested. For young children, these include nursery rhymes and rhyme games; making games with syllable beats in words; drawing attention to the printed word while reading to children; using books with rhymes and word patterns. Most importantly, parents need to be proactive by knowing what is happening in their child’s school program. Developing effective communication with teachers and therapists will help promote skill development and also help to identify potential roadblocks at the earliest possible time, before a significant problem has developed.

Children with spoken language problems that follow them into school need the proactive involvement of speech pathologists, teachers, and parents. Literacy related skills need to be carefully monitored. Children experiencing difficulty require a full language assessment to not only identify the problems but also to uncover the underlying reason for those problems in order for proper treatment to be outlined and delivered. The potential or risk for literacy related difficulties makes the jobs of both parents and therapists all the more challenged. While much of the focus for children with apraxia is necessarily on their speech production and oral motor sequencing skills, these children also need support and assistance throughout the course of therapy to assure that the proper groundwork is laid for developing literacy skills.

References for this paper:

Stackhouse, Joy (1997). Phonological awareness: Connecting speech and literacy problems. In B. Hodson and M.L. Edwards (Eds.), Perspectives in Applied Phonology (pp. 157 – 196). Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen Publications.

Snowling, M, & Stackhouse, J. (1983). Spelling Performance of Children with Developmental Verbal Dyspraxia. Developmental Medicine and Neurology, 25, 430 – 437.

Stackhouse, J. (1985). Segmentation, speech and spelling difficulties. In M. Snowling (Ed.), Children’s Written Language Difficulties (pp. 96 – 115). Philadelphia: NFER-Nelson Publishing.

Bridgeman, E. & Snowling, M. (1988). The perception of phoneme sequence: A comparison of dyspraxic and normal children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 23, 245 – 252.

30 years of research: How children learn to read. G. DeAngelis Sedlak (Ed.) SpeechJargon newsletter, Volume 2, Issue 4, November 1997.

30 years of research: What we know about how children learn to read, a synthesis of research on reading from N.I.C.H.D.

Graphomotor Skills: Why Some Kids Hate to Write

Why some kids hate to write, description of graphomotor skills.

Handwriting is complex perceptual-motor skill that is dependent upon the maturation and integration of a number of cognitive, perceptual and motor skills, and is developed through instruction (Hamstra-Bletz and Blote, 1993; Maeland, 1992). While a plethora of information exists in lay and professional literature about many of the common problems experienced by school age children, difficulty with handwriting is often overlooked and poorly understood. Students with graphomotor problems are frequently called “lazy”, “unmotivated” and/or “oppositional” because they are reluctant to produce written work. Many times, these are the children who dislike school the most. Because they are sometimes able to write legibly if they write slowly enough, they are accused of writing neatly “when they want to”. This statement has moral implications and is untrue; for children with graphomotor problems, neat handwriting at a reasonable pace is often not a choice.

When required to write, children with written production problems frequently engage in numerous avoidance behaviors. They have to go to the bathroom; they need to sharpen their pencils; they need a Kleenex from their backpack. Sometimes they just sit and stare. Even disrupting the class and getting in trouble may be less painful for them than writing. Work that could be completed in one hour takes three hours because they put off the dreadful task of writing.

The following paragraphs will attempt to elucidate the various components of handwriting and the characteristics which students display when there are breakdowns in these components. Components of graphomotor or handwriting skills include visual-perceptual skills, orthographic coding, motor planning and execution, kinesthetic feedback and visual-motor coordination.

Visual-Perceptual Skills.

Visual-perceptual skills enable children to visually discriminate among graphic forms and to judge their correctness. Thus, visual-perceptual skills involve the ability or capacity to accurately interpret or give meaning to what is seen. Generally a number of specific skills fall into this category including visual discrimination, or the ability to distinguish one visual pattern from another, and visual closure, or the ability to perceive a whole pattern when shown only parts of that pattern. Adequate visual-perceptual skills are a necessary but not sufficient condition for legible written output.

Orthographic Coding.

A second factor important to the production of legible handwriting is orthographic coding. Berninger and her colleagues (Berninger, Yates, Cartwright, Rutberg, Remy and Abbott, 1992) define orthographic coding as the “ability to represent a printed word in memory and then to access the whole word pattern, a single letter, or letter cluster in that representation” (pg. 260). Thus, orthographic coding refers to the ability to both store in memory and retrieve from memory letters and word patterns. The relationship between poor handwriting and orthographic coding deficits has been empirically established (Berninger et. al., 1992).

Motor Planning and Execution.

A third component of handwriting is praxis or the ability to plan and execute motor actions or behavior. Fitts and Posner (1967) describe motor skill acquisition as proceeding through three stages. The first phase is called the cognitive or early phase. In this phase, the learner establishes an understanding of the task and a cognitive map of the movements required to accomplish the task. In the second phase, the associated or intermediate phase, the movement patterns become more coordinated in time and space. During this phase, proprioceptive feedback (the feedback that the brain receives from the muscles and nerves) becomes increasingly important and the importance of visual feedback decreases. The final phase, the autonomous phase, is characterized by the development of larger functional units that are translated into a motor program which then occurs with minimal conscious attention.

Luria (1966) notes that a motor action begins with an idea about the purpose of an action and the possible ways in which this action may be performed. The ideas are stored as motor engrams. Thus, in order to carry out a motor behavior, we must have both the idea or image for what must be accomplished (i.e., the plan) and the ability to match our motor output to that plan. Therefore, both adequate motor planning and execution are necessary for handwriting. Levine (1987) includes in the definition of dyspraxia difficulty with assigning the various muscles or muscle groups to their roles in the writing task. This definition focuses on the execution or output aspect of dyspraxia. According to Levine, in order to hold a pencil effectively and produce legible handwriting at an acceptable rate, the fingers must hold the writing utensil in such a way that some fingers are responsible for stabilizing the pencil or pen and others are responsible for mobilizing it. In a normal tripod grasp, the index finger is responsible for stabilizing the writing instrument and the thumb and middle finger are responsible for the mobility of the instrument during writing.

Kinesthetic Feedback.

Yet another component of motor control for legible handwriting produced at an acceptable rate is feedback of the sensorimotor system, especially kinesthetic feedback, during the performance of motor actions. Luria (1966) points out that for effective motor action, there must be afferent impulses from the body to the brain that inform the brain about the location and movement of the body. The body then makes adjustments based on these impulses to alter its movement pattern until the desired pattern is achieved. Thus, it is kinesthetic feedback that facilitates a good match between the motor plan and motor execution. In writing, the writer has a kinesthetic plan in mind and compares this plan to the kinesthetic feedback and then either corrects, persists or terminates the graphomotor pattern (Levine, 1987).

Visual-Motor Coordination.

Visual-motor coordination is the ability to match motor output with visual input. Although it is the nonvisual or kinesthetic feedback that is crucial for handwriting, visual feedback is also important. Visual feedback provides gross monitoring of writing rather than the fine-tuned monitoring provided by nonvisual feedback. It is this gross monitoring that prevents us from writing on the desk, crossing over lines (Levine, 1987) and staying within the margins.

Problems with Graphomotor Skills

Deficits in visual-perceptual skills..

Children with visual-perceptual problems may have a history of reading problems because of difficulty with letter and word recognition. In addition, if a child cannot accurately visually discriminate the letter b from the letter d, he/she will be unable to reliably reproduce these letters upon demand. If students have problems with visual closure, they may have difficulty with accurate letter formation and handwriting legibility may be poor. For example, they may print the letter o with a space in the top, but perceive the letter as closed. When deficits in visual-perceptual skills are suspected, they can be readily identified by informal or standardized tests.

Deficits in Orthographic Coding.

Students who have trouble with orthographic coding will often forget how to form certain letters in the middle of a writing task. They frequently retrace letters or exhibit false starts or hesitancies as they write. Observations of their written output may show that they have formed the same letter several different ways. When asked, these students can usually report if they have difficulty remembering what letters look like. Children who cannot reliably make use of visual recall to form letters and words often prefer to print rather than write in cursive because print involves only twenty-six different visual letter patterns, whereas letters written in cursive have a seemingly endless number of visual patterns. Their spelling errors may be phonetic in nature (Levine, 1987, 1994).

Deficits in Motor Planning and Execution.

Poor motor planning and execution is referred to as dyspraxia. Deuel and Doar (1992) define dyspraxia as the “inability to learn or perform serial voluntary movements with the proficiency expected for age and/or verbal intelligence” (pg. 100). Helmer and Myklebust (1965) discuss the role that memory for motor sequences play in correctly forming letters when writing. Luria (1966) described two forms of dyspraxia. The first form involves difficulty in creating an image of a required motor movement. The second involves a breakdown in the central nervous system mechanism that is responsible for putting the plan into action. Thus, the child has the blueprint for the action/behavior, but has difficulty implementing it motorically (Levine, 1987).

Ayres (1972, 1975, 1985) suggested that the problem in developmental dyspraxia is in the neural activity that takes place prior to motor execution. According to Ayres, dyspraxia is generally viewed as an output problem because the motor component is more observable than the sensory component. However, in her view, dyspraxia is an inability to integrate sensory and motor information, rather than merely motor production.

Children who suffer from fine motor dyspraxia show poor motor coordination. At times, they assign too many muscles to stabilizing the pencil or pen and too few muscles to mobilizing it. At other times, they assign too many muscles to mobilizing the writing utensil and too few muscles to stabilizing it. Thus, their pencil grips are often inefficient. They may develop a hooked grip in which they stretch out the tendons in the back of the arm so that the fingers move very little if at all during writing. With this grip, they are using the larger muscles of the wrist and forearm which may be easier to control than the smaller muscles in the fingers. They often perform poorly with other fine motor tasks that involve coordinated motor movements such as tying shoes or holding a fork correctly (Levine, 1987).

Another pencil grip which suggests fine motor dyspraxia is one in which the child holds the pencil very tightly and near the point when writing. Further, students with dyspraxia often change pencil grips and prefer writing in cursive rather than print. They do not like to write and complain that their hand hurts when they write. Writing for them is a labor-intensive task. Fine motor dyspraxia is frequently associated with speech production problems because these children often have difficulty assigning the muscles in the mouth to specific speech sounds (Levine, 1987, 1994).

Impaired Kinesthetic Feedback.

Children with impaired kinesthetic feedback often develop a fist-like grip of the writing instrument. With this grip, they extend their thumb over the index and middle finger, limiting the mobility of the fingers. They may also press very hard on the paper with the writing utensil in an attempt to compensate for the lack of kinesthetic feedback. Further, they may look closely at the pencil or pen when writing thus attempting to guide the hand using visual feedback which is a much slower process. This is why children with impaired kinesthetic feedback may produce legible handwriting at a greatly reduced pace. As they progress in school, however, the demands placed on written output are too great and legibility deteriorates. These are the children who are often accused of writing neatly “when they want to”. They also often prefer to use mechanical pencils and “scratchy” pens because these provide more friction on the paper when writing. They complain that their hand hurts when writing and they do not like to write. Performance in other fine motor skills may be adequate or good because many fine motor skills do not place such reliance on kinesthetic feedback.

Research has shown that tasks which were designed to improve kinesthetic sensitivity improved handwriting performance more than a task that involved only practice in handwriting (Harris and Livesay, 1991).

Deficits in Visual-Motor Coordination.

Children with visual-motor incoordination function much differently than those with impaired kinesthetic feedback because of the different demands of certain motor tasks. Poor visual-motor integration may lead to problems with fine motor tasks that rely heavily on visual feedback. These include threading a needle, drawing, painting, craftwork, building things with blocks, repairing things, playing games such as Nintendo and using a mouse on a computer.

Strategies for Graphomotor Problems

  • For children who have difficulty with orthographic coding, it may be helpful to tape an alphabet line to the corner of their desk for easy reference.
  • Students with graphomotor problems should be given extended time to complete written assignments and/or a reduction in the volume of written output. For example, if the exercise given is to correctly capitalize and punctuate sentences or a passage, these should be provided to the student in typed form so that he/she has to only correct the work, rather than write it and then correct it. Also, if the assignment is to answer the questions at the end of the chapter in social studies, the student should be required only to write the answers, not both questions and answers. Additionally, he/she should be allowed to state answers in short phrases. In other words, if the subject matter being assessed is knowledge of information presented in the social studies chapter, it is this that should be assessed, not how competent the student is with the physical act of writing, or how much writing interferes with his/her ability to demonstrate his/her knowledge of social studies.
  • Children with handwriting difficulties may need to be given the opportunity to provide oral answers to exercises, quizzes, and tests.
  • Learning to type is helpful for these students. Writing assignments should be done in stages. Initially, the child would focus only on generating ideas. Next, he/she would organize his/her ideas. Finally, the student would attend to spelling and mechanical and grammatical rules. There are computer software programs available with spell and grammar checks.
  • Students with graphomotor problems may need to be provided with information presented on the board or on overheads in written form, such as teacher-prepared handouts or Xerox copies of other students’ notes.
  • Children with handwriting problems should be provided with written outlines so that they do not have to organize lectures or class materials themselves. This becomes particularly important in junior high grades.
  • Parents should be given the opportunity to purchase an extra set of textbooks for the purpose of highlighting, particularly for content area subjects. Also, notes may be made on Post-Its and then the Post-Its could be attached to a larger sheet.
  • It is often necessary to use alternative grading systems for children with graphomotor problems. One grade would be given for overall appearance and mechanics of writing, and the second for content.
  • When writing reports, it may be helpful for the student to identify his/her own errors and to correct these after learning specific strategies to do so. He/she would then list his/her most frequent errors in a workbook and refer to this list when self-correcting.
  • It should be stressed to school personnel that slow work habits are often a result of graphomotor difficulties and do not reflect deficits in motivation.
  • Electronic devices, such as the Franklin Speaking Spelling Ace may be helpful for students with handwriting problems.

Ayres, A. J. (1972). Sensory Integration and Learning Disorders. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Ayres, A. J. (1975). Sensorimotor foundations of academic ability. In W. Cruickshank & D. P. Hallahan (Eds.), Perceptual and learning disabilities in children: Volume 2, Research and Theory (pp. 300-360). New York: Syracuse University Press. Ayres, A. J. (1985). Developmental dyspraxia and adult onset apraxia. Torrance, CA: Sensory Integration International. Berninger, V., Yates, C., Cartwright, A., Rutberg, J., Remy, E., & Abbott, R. (1992). Lower-level developmental skills in beginning writing. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 257-280. Deuel, R.K., & Doar, B.P. (1992). Developmental manual dyspraxia: A lesson in mind and brain. Journal of Child Neurology, 7, 99-103. Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human Performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole. Hamstra-Bletz, L., & Blote, A.W. (1993). A longitudinal study on dysgraphic handwriting in primary school. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 689-699. Harris, S.J., & Livesey, D.J. (1992). Improving handwriting through kinesthetic sensitivity practice. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 39, 23-27. Levine, M.D. (1987). Developmental Variation and Learning Disorders. Educators Publishing Service, Inc.: Cambridge, Massachusetts. Levine, M.D. (1994). Educational Care: A System for Understanding and Helping Children with Learning Problems at Home and in School. Educators Publishing Service, Inc.: Cambridge, Massachusetts. Luria, A.R. (1966). Higher Cortical Functions in Man. Basic Books, Inc.: New York. Maeland, A.F. (1992). Handwriting and perceptual-motor skills in clumsy, dysgraphic, and ënormalí children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 1207-1217. Myklebust, H.R. (1965). Developmental disorders of written language: Vol. 1. Picture story language test. Grune & Stratton: New York.

Dr. Thorne is Vice President of Clinical Services for the Center for Development and Learning (CDL) in Louisiana. CDL specializes in the development and dissemination of leading edge reasearch, knowledge, training and best practices from diverse yet related fields that impact educational success. Visit CDL’s website at: https://www.cdl.org/

Language Processing and Comprehension Issues and Children with CAS

Language processing refers to the mental operations by which we perceive, recognize, understand and remember sounds, words, and sentences. Because it happens “inside the head,” language processing can’t be seen directly, instead, we have to test for processing problems.

It’s natural to focus on the speech production difficulties of children with CAS, but there are good reasons for parents and clinicians to take a careful look at their comprehension and processing. For one thing, speaking and understanding are tightly linked; in fact, one influential theory suggests that our knowledge of speech movements helps us perceive speech sounds. For another, children who have processing and comprehension problems often “get by” in everyday situations by using their knowledge and previous experiences to help them understand at least when they are young. But as they get older, they face more and more situations in which there are no extra clues to comprehension, and even mild processing difficulties can slow down their understanding and interfere with their performance.

Some “symptoms” of comprehension and processing problems:

  • The child may understand single words, and have an excellent vocabulary, but have difficulties in understanding phrases or sentences. Because comprehension can be so variable, others may think that the child is “just not trying” or “not paying attention.”
  • The child’s understanding will likely be better in everyday situations than in situations where there are few or no extra clues to meaning. In such situations, the child may fail to respond, may repeatedly say “Huh?”, may simply guess what has been asked, or may even repeat some or all of what was said.
  • Lengthy, complex, and abstract sentences are especially difficult for children with processing problems, especially if presented at normal or faster speaking rates.

If your child has normal hearing but you suspect a processing problem, a speech-language pathologist can assess his or her skills in single-word and sentence comprehension, as well as in phonological processing and phonological memory.

Some suggestions for parents whose children have processing problems:

  • Draw your child’s attention to speech sounds in words, using rhyming and silly sound games. Point out how new sounds and words are similar to and different from sounds and words your child knows well.
  • Don’t pretend you understand when you don’t. Show your child that communication breakdowns happen to everybody, and that people have to work together to fix them. Children who are at least 5 years old can be taught to monitor their comprehension, and to know what to do when they fail to understand.
  • When possible, be face-to-face when talking to your child. Speaking at an unhurried rate, repeating key words and important information.
  • Try not to give too much information at once. Rather than saying, “Get your coat on because we’re going to stop by Grandma’s house to pick up her dry cleaning after we pick up some things from the grocery story and pick up your brother from soccer,” you can say, “Let’s get your coat on. We have 3 things to do today. First, we are going to pick up your brother from soccer. Then I need to go to the grocery store to pick up food for our dinner. After that, we can stop by Grandma’s to pick up her dry cleaning.”
  • Children who have difficulty with language processing and comprehension may become frustrated or worry about disappointing people when they are given instructions to follow that are too long or complex. You can simplify your instructions by giving one or two instructions at a time rather than a big list of things to do. When you give instructions, you can do a quick comprehension check by asking your child to retell the instructions. You also can ask your child to check in with you after they have completed an instruction. For instance, if you’re taking your child to the park, but they need to do a few things before you leave, you can tell your child, “We’re going to the park. Please put your puzzles back on the shelf. Where do your puzzles go?” “ On the shelf. ” That’s right. Let me know when you’re done… Great! Thanks for cleaning up your puzzles. Go upstairs and put on the shorts and t-shirt I set on your bed. Let me know when you’re dressed… Oh good. Wash your hands, and then we can go to the park.”
  • When reading books, stop periodically and ask questions or have short discussions about what you’re reading. If possible, make connections between what you’re reading and your child’s life. Some books for children may contain complex sentence structures and sophisticated vocabulary. Consider rewording complicated sentences while reading aloud to your child. Don’t hesitate to stop and explain the meaning of words that may be unfamiliar to your child.

(Chris Dollaghan, Ph.D., CCC-SLP, is a professor in the Department of Communication Science and Disorders at the University of Pittsburgh, where she teaches and conducts research in pediatric speech and language disorders. She also previously served as chairperson of ASHA’s Research and Scientific Affairs Committee.)

Education, Learning, and Academics and Your Child with CAS

Parents have many questions about what will happen when their child becomes school age.  Can my child attend preschool? What kind of preschool?  Should my child go to kindergarten this year?  Educational placements for children with apraxia can cause parents a lot of stress.  In a nutshell, educational settings for children with apraxia of speech are just as varied as the children themselves!  There is no one “right” type of classroom or school setting that is perfect for all children with CAS.

What is Special Education?

Special education refers to laws, policies, rules, and very importantly – services for children who are determined to have a disability and who, because of their disability, are in need of specially designed instruction and related services in order to benefit from their educational program.

Special Education Rights, Rules, and Procedures.

Learning about special education rights is crucial for your participation in your child’s educational planning and to prepare you to be your child’s best advocate.  This is no easy feat given the busy lives we lead insuring that our children are receiving speech therapy and possibly other therapies, raising other children, working, etc.  However, it is well worth the effort because it is the only way you as the parent will be able to fully insure that your child is receiving what they are entitled to receive – a free, appropriate education.  Also, learning what the law says about special education rights may also apply to parents who are planning to send their children to private schools and/or home-school.  If you in any way plan to use public services to assist in your child’s educational development, it is critically important for you to pursue knowledge and understanding of your child’s special education rights.

There are many internet resources that provide quite detailed information on special education rights under federal law and as interpreted through specific state regulations in your state.  Apraxia-KIDS can help you locate appropriate sources of information to begin that learning process.  However, following is a review of some generally important concepts.

Evaluation for Special Education Eligibility and Services.

Most children with significant apraxia of speech, after evaluation by the school district, will be eligible for special education services, or, depending on which state you live in, a special education unit.  Speech therapy is one of many related services that can be provided through special education.  Support to children for their special education needs is based on a child’s unique needs and abilities.  There is no “one size fits all” when it comes to children that are eligible for special education.

In order to receive preschool or school-age special education services, the school district or designated appropriate agency has to evaluate your child in all areas of suspected disabilities.  If a child has been enrolled in a birth to three early intervention program, the planning for transition to preschool should occur prior to the child’s third birthday.  The school system will gather past and current information and will then, with your signed consent, create an evaluation plan to fully evaluate your child.  Parents may contact their school district Special Education Administrator or school principal to request that their child be evaluated for special education.  Putting the request for evaluation in writing is the best strategy to assure that a response is provided in a timely manner.

As the parent, you have a right to know what tests will be administered to your child.  Please be aware that some tests would be invalid and inappropriate for a child with low or limited verbal skills, including many tests of cognitive or intellectual abilities.  Also, testing by the school for eligibility for special education is free to you.  At the end of the evaluation process, you will receive an Evaluation Report that will share the results and interpretation of scores, strengths and needs, recommendations regarding whether or not your child is eligible for special education, and if eligible, what types of goals and services may be required to meet your child’s individual education needs.

Individual Education Planning (IEP)

After an evaluation is complete, a meeting should occur to review the findings of the evaluation and begin to identify a number of things about your child, including:

  • His/her present level of functioning (what CAN your child currently do, and what CAN’T your child do).  This includes your child’s strengths, needs, learning style.
  • Based on present level of functioning, the creation of clear and objectively measurable goals for the child to achieve in one years time (annual goals)
  • Steps to achieving the goals (short-term objectives or benchmarks)
  • Types of supports and services the child needs to achieve the goals and objectives
  • Amount of services to be provided and method of delivering the services.

When the above steps are in place, the IEP team should discuss classroom placement.  Parents should be aware that the point of special education is to meet your child’s individual educational needs by providing appropriate supports and services.   A plan is to be designed around your child’s educational needs, not around a predetermined classroom and what services are reported to be available.  Your child’s needs must be the focus. Thus be wary if you are told that your school district doesn’t provide a service (for example, “we only provide group speech therapy”).  A policy that would limit the available service delivery for speech therapy would be frankly illegal.  Again, the kinds of services and ways that they are to be delivered must be based on the child’s individual needs and not on convenience, budget, or preference of school systems.

Some things to know or remember:

  • All children with apraxia of speech are unique individuals and have unique needs.  The children do share a speech diagnosis, and thus, are likely to require frequent and intensive speech therapy for a period of time, however; other speech, language or learning needs are often present in many children.  In the special education process, try to focus on your particular child’s strengths and needs vs. comparing them to another child who may or may not have the exact same issues.
  • Ask questions for clarification
  • Retell stories using appropriate grammar and sequence of events
  • Use correct vocabulary and word usage when speaking
  • Ask and answer appropriate questions and share personal experiences within a group
  • Respond to and initiate conversations

Each of the areas above would be important at any grade level and would be made very challenging for a child with a significant speech disorder!  Understanding what standards your state expects for children of your child’s age and grade level can help you advocate for an appropriate education plan and services.

  • Children with apraxia often have areas of concern that are in addition to speech production or speaking ability.  For example, children with apraxia of speech often have expressive and/or receptive language difficulties; reading, writing or spelling challenges, etc.  It is the school’s responsibility to assure that your child is evaluated in all areas of suspected disability.
  • It is helpful to start with the idea that your child’s school staff and you, the parent, can work effectively in partnership with one another.  That should be a main goal because your child will benefit when that sort of partnership occurs!  While it may not always be possible, we observe that time and time again children with apraxia benefit immensely when educators, professionals, and families are able to unite behind what a child needs and work together.
  • The special education process can be very overwhelming, confusing, and scary!  There are organizations in each state that are funded by the federal government to assure that parents receive help and training in understanding their child’s rights and the special education process.  These entities are called “Parent Training and Information Centers.”  To find one in your state, go to Google.com and put “parent training and information centers and special education” in the search.

Education Issues for Children with Apraxia

There has been recent research that indicates children with a diagnosis of childhood apraxia of speech (and other certain speech sound disorders) are at high risk for literacy problems and language-learning related educational difficulties. Speech-language pathologists in the school systems are instrumental in helping educators understand the ramifications of speech/language difficulties in the realm of literacy related activities. Phonological awareness, reading, spelling and written expression are identified in the literature as possible problem areas. Additionally, comprehension and overall language processing are other possible deficit areas for monitoring and remediation. The SLP can participate with educators to select appropriate materials that address a child’s main weakness. For example, explicit training in letter – sound associations or systematic, multisensory reading approaches best serve some children with apraxia.

Children with CAS often have poor oral and written language and narrative skills. Social and pragmatic language skills also necessarily suffer when there is a deficit in narrative skills. Many children with CAS have had less opportunity to participate in conversation due to their severe speech production challenges and thus have difficulty developing these important skills. Careful attention, evaluation, and remediation of such skills warrant the earliest possible intervention. A great deal of future success both in school and in life has at its base adequate pragmatic language ability.

Related Articles

relationship between speech reading and writing

Individualized Education Plans (IEPs)

Individualized Education Plans (IEPs) Speech and Language Goals When Planning...

Educational Placement and Your Child with CAS

Educational Placement and Your Child with CAS Overall, the public...

Retain or Promote? Making Educational Decisions for Your Child with Apraxia

Retain or Promote? Making Educational Decisions for Your Child with...

Two young girls read books at a table in a classroom.

The power of touch is vital for both reading and writing

relationship between speech reading and writing

Professor Emerita of Linguistics, American University

Disclosure statement

Naomi S. Baron does not work for, consult, own shares in or receive funding from any company or organisation that would benefit from this article, and has disclosed no relevant affiliations beyond their academic appointment.

American University provides funding as a member of The Conversation US.

View all partners

“ Pat the Bunny ,” the 1940 classic touch-and-feel book, is still in print – a testament to the value of touch in introducing infants and toddlers to the world of reading. Later, when children reach school age, a common technique for teaching the alphabet is using hands-on manipulation, such as forming letters out of clay.

But as these students get older, the role of touch diminishes – to the students’ detriment. Today’s reading assignments are heavily digital , and use of computer keyboards for writing shows no sign of abating, especially given the lure of AI tools for editing and composing.

I’m a linguist who investigates the differences between print and digital reading and how writing supports thinking . My colleague Anne Mangen and I asked more than 500 secondary students at an international school in Amsterdam about their experiences when reading print versus digital texts. Separately, I surveyed 100 university students and young adults in the U.S. and Europe on their likes and dislikes about handwriting versus typing.

Together, their responses demonstrate that adolescents and young adults continue to value touch in their encounters with the written word. The research offers important lessons for educators and parents.

What students tell us

In the studies, students wrote glowingly about touch when asked for the one thing they liked most about reading in print or writing by hand. What surprised me was how closely their perceptions about the importance of touch aligned in both studies.

On a physical level, the feeling of holding a book or writing instrument in their hands mattered to students. These are some of their observations: “You actually feel like you are reading because the book is in your hands,” and “I like feeling the paper and pen under my hands, being able to physically form words.” Study participants also commented on the interaction of touch and movement. Regarding reading, one wrote about “the feeling of turning each page and anticipating what’s to happen next.” About writing by hand, one participant described “being able to feel the words just glide across the page.”

Many students also mentioned cognitive benefits. A host of respondents wrote about focus, concentration, immersion or memory. Regarding print reading, one student said, “I take it more seriously because it’s physically in my hands.” For writing, one response was, “I can see what I’m thinking.”

There were also psychological reflections. Students wrote, “The feeling of a book in my hands is a very comfortable feeling,” and “The satisfaction of a whole page filled by handwriting, it feels like I climbed a mountain.” Other comments addressed how touch made students feel more personally connected to the act of reading and writing. About reading, one reflected that “it is more personal ‘cause it’s in your hands.” About handwriting, another declared, “I feel more attached to the content I produce.”

A number of respondents wrote that reading physical books and writing by hand somehow felt more “real” than engaging with their digital counterparts. One student commented on “the realness of the book.” Another reported that “it feels more real than writing on a computer, the words seem to have more meaning.”

The studies also asked what participants liked most about digital reading and about writing on a computer keyboard. Out of more than 600 answers, only one mentioned the role of touch in what they liked most about using these technologies for reading and writing. For reading, students praised the convenience and access to the internet. For writing, greater speed as well as internet access were frequent responses.

Girls sit a table working on touchscreen tablets.

What science tells us

What students say about the importance of touch mirrors what researchers have found: Touch is an effective way to build early reading and writing skills , as well as to support how more developed readers and writers interact with the written word .

Psychologists and reading specialists continue to report higher comprehension in children and young adults when reading in print versus digitally, for both academic and leisure reading . For proficient writers, evidence suggests that spending more time writing by hand than using a computer keyboard correlates with better fine motor skills . A recent study in Norway compared brain images of university students taking notes and found that those who wrote by hand – rather than typing – showed greater electrical connectivity in the parts of the brain that process new information and support memory formation.

Strategies going forward

The challenge for teachers and parents is to figure out how to incorporate touch into literacy activities in a world that’s so reliant on digital tools. Here are three suggestions for addressing this paradox.

• Parents and teachers can begin by listening to students themselves. Despite all their time spent on digital devices, many young people clearly recognize how touch contributes to their reading and writing experiences. Expand the conversation by talking together about differences between digital and hands-on reading and writing.

• Next, parents can find opportunities for children to read print and write by hand outside of school, such as bringing their kids to the library to check out print books and encouraging them to write a story or keep a journal at home. Better still is when adults model these practices in their own lives.

• Finally, educators need to increase space in the curriculum for print reading and for handwritten assignments. Some teachers are already revisiting the intrinsic benefits of handwriting , including as a memory aid and a vehicle for thinking – both qualities that participants mentioned in my writing survey.

Digital reading materials and keyboards will undoubtedly persist in schools and homes. But this reality must not preclude the power of touch.

  • Handwriting
  • Digital age
  • K-12 education

relationship between speech reading and writing

Events and Communications Coordinator

relationship between speech reading and writing

Assistant Editor - 1 year cadetship

relationship between speech reading and writing

Executive Dean, Faculty of Health

relationship between speech reading and writing

Lecturer/Senior Lecturer, Earth System Science (School of Science)

relationship between speech reading and writing

Sydney Horizon Educators (Identified)

IMAGES

  1. The Reading/Writing Connection

    relationship between speech reading and writing

  2. The reading writing literacy connection

    relationship between speech reading and writing

  3. 16.5: The Major Channels of Management Communication Are Talking

    relationship between speech reading and writing

  4. Tips & Guides

    relationship between speech reading and writing

  5. Relationship And Difference Between Speech And Writing In Linguistics

    relationship between speech reading and writing

  6. Reading and Writing Connections

    relationship between speech reading and writing

VIDEO

  1. Four Skills of Communication| Listening, Speaking, Reading & Writing| Process and Barrier to Avoid

  2. How Reading Improves Writing

  3. What is the Difference between speech and debate?

  4. Why Reading Books Makes Us Better At Speaking #andrewhuberman #neuroscience

  5. Listening Comprehension and Booktalk: What Does the Research Tell Us?

  6. Why music is not a language

COMMENTS

  1. The Relation between Speech and Reading

    The association between speech and reading is mediated mainly, but not exclusively, by. genetic factors (Gillam, 2008; Hayiou‐Thomas, 2010; Shipley & McFarlane 1981). Reading/learning disabilities have been identified primarily on the basis of reading problems.

  2. The Symbiotic Relationship Between Reading and Writing

    Explaining the Symbiotic Relationship Between Reading and Writing. Students who understand how reading relates to writing and vice versa can develop into better writers. For elementary school teachers, the saying is, students learn to read and then read to learn. At the middle and high school levels, teachers may experience the relationship of ...

  3. Speaking and Writing Interconnections: A Systematic Review

    Abstract. Drawing on previous studies on the relationship between speaking and writing modalities, this paper provides a critical synthesis of theoretical and empirical research on the ...

  4. Linguistics: Between Orality and Writing

    Interactions and relationships between speech and writing replace notions of stark transitions and divides. The example of oral composition, performance, and recording in antiquity is seminal. ... and sometimes read it with reference to Heath's silences rather than to her rich ethnographies of speech, writing, and reading. Yet the book ...

  5. Reading Risk in Children With Speech Sound Disorder: Prevalence

    Relationship between speech-sound disorders and early literacy skills in preschool-age children: Impact of comorbid language impairment. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics , 28(6), 438-447.

  6. 4 Ways Reading and Writing Interlock: What the Research Says

    1. Reading and writing are intimately connected. Research on the connections between the two disciplines began in the early 1980s and has grown more robust with time. Although there are elements ...

  7. PDF DOI :https://doi.org/10.48185/jtls.v2i2.280 Speaking and Writing

    p. 127). In other words, the theory holds that writing depends on speech. "Unidirectional model", therefore, focuses on correspondences and similarities between speech and writing because writing is thought to represent spoken language. With regards to the correspondences from speech to writing, transcribing speech sounds into

  8. The Shifting Relationships between Speech and Writing

    The Traditional View: Indelible Writing, Ephemeral Speech. Obviously writing is more indelible or permanent than speech. Speech is nothing but wind, waves of temporarily squashed air, waves that begin at once to disperse, that is, to lose their sound. Writing, on the other hand, stays there-"down in black and white."

  9. What Oral/Written Language Differences Can Tell Us about Beginning

    between speech and writing. "What kids need to learn is that print is speech written down." "Teachers need to build up the children's oral language before attempting to teach them to read and write." "Literacy is built on an oral language base." These near truisms for the beginning reading and writing teacher are famil iar to all of us.

  10. PDF Are reading and writing building on the same skills? The relationship

    language user becomes more proficient in reading and writing. To summarize, we will investigate the extent to which the relationship between reading and writing ability can be explained by language resources they both appeal to, and we will do so for reading and writing in L1 and EFL, and at three stages of development, respectively.

  11. Written Language Disorders

    A disorder of written language involves a significant impairment in fluent word reading (i.e., reading decoding and sight word recognition), reading comprehension, written spelling, and/or written expression (Ehri, 2000; Gough & Tunmer, 1986; Kamhi & Catts, 2012; Tunmer & Chapman, 2007, 2012). A word reading disorder is also known as dyslexia.

  12. Relationship Between Speech-Sound Disorders and Early Literacy Skills

    Hypothesis 1: Association Between Severity of Speech-Sound Disorder and Early Reading/Writing Readiness. Contrary with our hypothesis, when adjusting for the child's sex and SES, the severity of SSD was positively associated with the TERA (p < .05) but not the TEWL (p = .15). However, this association was no longer significant when controlling ...

  13. On the relationship between speech and writing with implications for

    Two theories of the relationship between speech and writing are examined. One theory holds that writing is restricted to a one-way relationship with speech—a unidirectional influence from speech to writing. In this theory, writing is derived from speech and is simply a representation of speech. The other theory holds that additional, multidirectional influences are involved in the ...

  14. How Do Speaking and Writing Support Each Other?

    Structurally, there are also differences between speech and writing. In speech, fo r e x am pl e, cl au se s a re li nk e d b y s im pl e c on ju nc ti on s s uc h a s and , but , so

  15. On the relationship between speech and writing with implications for

    One theory holds that writing is restricted to a one-way relationship with speech-a unidirectional influence from speech to writing. In this theory, writing is derived from speech and is simply a representation of speech. The other theory holds that additional, multidirectional influences are involved in the development of writing.

  16. The Link Between Speech Sound Disorders and Reading/Writing Disorders

    Speech sound disorders (SSDs) exist along a spectrum of severity and abilities, with many involving both the motoric and the phonological system. As a result, many children with SSDs experience related issues with the phonological skills needed for word reading and spelling. This session reviews the Simple Views of Reading and of Writing and connects those theories to assessment practices.

  17. Relationship And Difference Between Speech And Writing In Linguistics

    Some other differences between the two are as follows: Relationship and Difference Between Speech and Writing in Linguistics Permanency. Writing is more permanent than the spoken word and is changed less easily. Once something is printed, or published on the internet, it is out there for the world to see permanently.

  18. The Relationship between English Speaking and Writing Proficiency and

    The Relationship between English Speaking and Writing Proficiency and Its Implications for Instruction Pamela Rausch St. Cloud State University Follow this and additional works at:https://repository.stcloudstate.edu/engl_etds This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of English at theRepository at St. Cloud State.

  19. The Relationship Between Reading and Writing

    Basically put: reading affects writing and writing affects reading. According to recommendations from the major English/Language Arts professional organizations, reading instruction is most effective when intertwined with writing instruction and vice versa. Research has found that when children read extensively they become better writers.

  20. On the relationship between speech and writing with implications for

    One theory holds that writing is restricted to a one-way relationship with speech—a unidirectional influence from speech to writing. In this theory, writing is derived from speech and is simply a representation of speech. The other theory holds that additional, multidirectional influences are involved in the development of writing.

  21. The Relationship between Reading and Writing

    Abstract. Writing and speaking are basically different activities, and they must be separately developed. The first step in learning to write is learning to read. The natural way to improve one's writing is to cultivate the habit of reading for pleasure. Reading is the easiest, fastest, most convenient, most enjoyable, and most generally ...

  22. PDF A Relationship between Reading and Writing: The Conversational Model

    The conversational model points up the fact that writing occurs within the con- text of previous writing and advances the total sum of the discourse. Earlier com- ments provide subjects at issue, factual content, ideas to work with, and models of discourse appropriate to the subject. Later comments build on what came before and may, therefore ...

  23. Children with Apraxia and Reading, Writing, and Spelling Difficulties

    Many parents wonder if their young child with apraxia of speech (verbal dyspraxia) will go on to experience difficulties in their education .While there is no certainty that literacy problems will or will not develop, there is research that has shown that children with spoken language problems are at higher risk for literacy related problems.The purpose of this paper is to summarize some ...

  24. The power of touch is vital for both reading and writing

    A number of respondents wrote that reading physical books and writing by hand somehow felt more "real" than engaging with their digital counterparts. One student commented on "the realness ...